Abstract
Objective
To determine the relationship between line bisection test (LBT) performance time and prognosis of hemispatial neglect (HSN) in stroke patients.
Methods
Data on stroke patients with HSN were prospectively collected. After patient recruitment and eligibility screening, the LBT, Motor-Free Visual Perception Test 3rd edition, and Korean version of Mini-Mental State Examination were performed at the time of admission and 4 weeks thereafter. The LBT performance time was also measured. All patients received conventional rehabilitation for 4 weeks. Based on the improvements in their LBT grades, the patients were divided into improved and non-improved groups. The evaluation results of the two groups were compared using Mann–Whitney U-tests and logistic regression was performed to predict the independence of each outcome.
Results
In total, 26 stroke patients with HSN were included, with 13 patients in each group. Significant differences were observed in the baseline LBT performance times between the improved and non-improved groups (p<0.05). Logistic regression analysis revealed associations between HSN prognosis, and baseline LBT performance time (odds ratio=0.95; 95% confidence interval, 0.90–1.00; p<0.05) and baseline Motor-Free Visual Perception Test 3rd edition (odds ratio=1.20; 95% confidence interval, 1.01–1.43; p<0.05).
Hemispatial neglect (HSN) is characterized by reduced awareness of stimuli from the opposite side of the brain lesion [1]. HSN is closely related to damage to the temporoparietal junction and the posterior parietal cortex of the brain area which is associated with attention to the contralateral space. The lack of attention in the contralateral space can occur in the visual, auditory, proprioceptive, and olfactory areas, and is characterized by a lack of awareness of sensory input [2]. HSN is not uncommon in patients with right hemispheric lesions, occurring in 13%–81% of cases [3,4]. HSN patients experience difficulties in performing daily activities and may not be able to detect dangerous objects located on the affected side [5]. These issues can make post-stroke rehabilitation difficult and prevent functional recovery [6]. Therefore, HSN diagnosis and prognosis prediction are important for achieving rehabilitation [7].
The evaluation of HSN in stroke patients is important and various tests have been developed for this purpose [8]. The most commonly used pencil-and-paper tests are the line bisection test (LBT) [9], clock drawing test [10], Albert test [11], and star cancellation test [12]. In particular, the LBT is a simple and less time-consuming test that is widely used in the clinical setting, with a relative test sensitivity of 76.4% for visuospatial neglect [13]. However, grade assessment by LBT in HSN includes only the deviation from the evaluation result and the performance time is not considered [14]. HSN after stroke is significantly improved in 43% of cases within 2 weeks and 63% of cases are nearly recovered within 3 months [15]. HSN is associated with various cognitive functions in the patients; therefore, many studies have assessed the relationships between neglect and various cognitive functions [16]. Previous studies demonstrated the relationship between neglect and cognitive impairment using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), while the severity of neglect was assessed using visual perception tests such as the MotorFree Visual Perception Test (MVPT) [17,18]. HSN patients reportedly have greater deterioration in attention [19,20], which is related to the duration of the cognitive function test and test performance time [21,22]. The present study investigated whether LBT performance time could predict the HSN prognosis and to confirm the relationship between MMSE and MVPT-3 (MVPT 3rd edition).
We prospectively enrolled all patients with a first acute stroke admitted to the Department of Rehabilitation Medicine of CHA Bundang Medical Center between December 2017 and March 2019. Patients were included: (1) who experienced a stroke as confirmed by a neurologist using computed tomography or magnetic resonance imaging, (2) were aged ≥20 years, (3) were able to perform at least a one-step obey command, (4) had motor power of at least one upper limb higher than a fair grade on the manual muscle test on admission (as fair and below fair motor grades could interfere with the evaluation test), and (5) who were diagnosed with HSN using the LBT. Subjects (1) with an underlying neurological condition other than stroke such as brain tumor, traumatic brain injury, and hydrocephalus; (2) with a history of diseases that can affect visual function such as ophthalmologic disease; and (3) in whom evaluation was impossible because they could not sit independently were excluded. The study was conducted according to the principles of the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of CHA Bundang Medical Center (No. CHAMC2017-11-037). All patients were provided written and oral information about the study and all patients provided their consent before study participation.
All patients received neglect treatment such as visual scanning, prism adaptation, biofeedback training, and limb activation depending on their function. Five patients received repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for cognitive improvement (Table 1).
Based on the improvement in LBT grade (from that at baseline to that at 4 weeks thereafter), the patients were divided into improved and non-improved groups. The patients were allocated to the improved group when the LBT grade was lower than that at baseline (that measured at admission); if the baseline LBT grade was not lower than that at baseline, the patient was allocated to the non-improved group.
All the evaluations were performed by an experienced occupational therapist and reviewed by a rehabilitation specialist. The following evaluation tests were performed 4 weeks after the baseline evaluation.
The LBT is a tool proposed by Schenkenberg et al. [9] for evaluating unilateral neglect. On the test paper are 20 lines in a random arrangement, with 8 lines in the center, 6 on the left, and 6 on the right. The lines are 100, 120, 140, 160, 180, and 200 mm in length (Fig. 1). In this study, the A4-size test paper was placed on the patient’s center line with the patient in a sitting position. The examiner first showed the center point of the two top and bottom lines and instructed the patient to mark the center points of the remaining 18 lines. The top and bottom demonstration lines were excluded when measuring scores. The test ended when the patient believed that he/she had completed the task and placed the pen on the desk. In this study, the test time was measured using a stopwatch and there was no time limit.
The severity of HSN was classified as follows in this study. We calculated the mean of the 6 left and 6 center straight lines and calculated the standard deviation for each patient using the following formula:
The mean value of normal subjects was -0.59, with a standard deviation of 4.35. A standard deviation absolute value >l indicated mild neglect (grade 1), >2 indicated moderate neglect (grade 2), and >3 indicated severe neglect (grade 3). Patients with a standard deviation absolute value <1 were classified as normal [14].
The MVPT-3 is a tool for evaluating visual perception ability without using motor function (Fig. 1). The MVPT was first developed in the 1970s; the present study used the MVPT-3 revision from 2003. The preliminary reliability and validity have been reported and its validity has been reported using Rasch analysis [23,24]. The MVPT-3 assesses the overall perception of spatial relationships, visual discrimination, figure-ground discrimination, visual closure, and visual memory, and is divided into the following eight sub-areas: visual discrimination, form constancy, visual short-term memory 1, visual closure 1, spatial orientation, figure ground, visual closure 2, and visual short-term memory [23]. The test comprises 65 items; for patients aged ≥11 years, only items 14–65 are evaluated. In this study, the raw score was used in the evaluation without using the conversion score according to age.
Of several MMSE translations used in Korea, this study used the K-MMSE. The K-MMSE comprises five points of orientation for time (5 points), orientation for place (5 points), registration (3 points), attention and calculation (5 points), recall (3 points), language (8 points), and visual construction (1 point). The total score is 30 points [25]. In this study, the differences between the scores at baseline and after 4 weeks were verified using the total score.
Differences in the baseline characteristic variables between the patient groups were analyzed by age, onset to baseline evaluation, Korean version of the Modified Barthel Index score using Mann–Whitney U-tests, sex, and etiology using Fisher exact tests; the side and location of the brain lesions were analyzed using linear by linear association. Mann–Whitney U-tests were conducted to compare the baseline and changes in LBT time, MVPT-3, and K-MMSE between the two groups.
Logistic regression analysis was conducted to identify the prognostic factors of HSN from baseline. The included factors were age, sex, onset to baseline evaluation day, side of the brain lesion, location of the brain lesion (frontal, parietal, and temporal lobe), baseline LBT time, baseline MVPT-3 score, and baseline MMSE score.
Statistical analyses were performed using standard statistical software (SPSS version 21.0 for Windows; IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). The significance level was set at below 0.05.
A total of 39 patients were recruited. Six were lost to follow-up; of the remaining patients, 7 who received grade 0 in the baseline LBT were excluded from the statistical analysis (Fig. 2). Finally, 26 stroke patients (18 men and 8 women; mean age, 60.4±10.5 years) with HSN were included (15 with ischemic stroke and 11 with hemorrhagic stroke). According to the change in the LBT grade, the patients were divided into the improved (n=13) and non-improved (n=13) groups. The times from onset to baseline evaluation were 56.6±122.2 days in the improved group and 27.0±18.6 days in the non-improved group. All patients showed the times from onset to baseline evaluation within 90 days, except for one patient in the improved group (460 days). Table 1 summarizes the demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients. No statistically significant differences were observed between the two groups.
Logistic regression analysis performed for each variable in all the subjects showed that sex, baseline LBT time, and baseline MVPT-3 score were significantly associated with HSN (Table 3).
This study aimed to determine whether the LBT performance time, which corresponds to sustained attention, is related to HSN prognosis. The results demonstrated a shorter baseline LBT performance time, higher proportion of male sex, and higher baseline MVPT-3 score in the improved group. Therefore, HSN was more likely to reduce when the test performance time was short because the LBT was conducted more intensively.
Many theories have been proposed to explain the characteristic behaviors of HSN, the most widely accepted of which is the attention-based theory. HSN is believed to be caused by an abnormality in the brain network that results in a lack of attention control [26,27]. Therefore, the maintenance of attention is accompanied by lower arousal and speed processing; this not only reduces task performance but also decreases the response to the stimulus at rest [27,28]. The present study measured the test performance time to confirm patient attention. The LBT is a simple and clinically useful test for HSN for which various detailed test methods and analyses have been developed [29]. However, in general, there is little consideration of the time concept when analyzing the LBT. Most studies use the average of several straight lines; however, the number, length, and position of the lines on the sheet of paper differs lightly. Some studies have used the concept of time to interpret results, in which performance time >90 seconds is indicative of severe-grade neglect [30]. The introduction of this time concept may be useful to determine the HSN severity.
Various HSN assessment tools have been developed in which various tasks are performed and evaluated. One assessment of the method of interpretation contains the concept of time (to the extent that it is performed with a time limit); however, most methods do not use the concept of time [31]. In a study using a cancellation test conducted on a touch screen, the HSN group was slower and less efficient than the control group [32]. Among patients with the same degree of severity in the previous assessment, patient who with severe loss of concentration may have a delayed performance time. Therefore, incorporation of the time concept in popularized tests such as the LBT and star calculation tests may allow a more comprehensive assessment. HSN can be accompanied by cognitive impairments in attention, memory, visual perception, and visual-motor organization [33]. Previous studies reported that patients with poor outcomes of HSN had significantly lower visual perception and memory function [34] and chronic HSN was related to sustained inattention [35]. In our study, shorter baseline LBT performance time and higher baseline MVPT-3 results had a more positive effect on patient outcomes. Therefore, cognitive impairment such as attention and visual perception affects HSN recovery in patients.
HSN patients may miss or perceive distorted information in the visual hemifield on the opposite side of the lesion during evaluation. This phenomenon makes most perceptual tests difficult for HSN patients. Some studies have developed a version of the MVPT with vertically aligned answers to eliminate these effects [36]. Rather, these tests remove the HSN variable that involves the disadvantage of not being usable for evaluating activities of daily living such as driving [37]. Our results showed no significant differences in the baseline MVPT-3 scores of both groups. However, the logistic regression results indicated that the higher the baseline MVPT-3 score, the better the prognosis. Thus, patients with the same HSN severity who had worse prognoses are expected to have lower baseline MVPT-3 scores.
The MMSE was originally developed to diagnose dementia and has a variety of cognitive subdomains [38,39]. In previous studies, patients with neglect mostly showed decreased time orientation, attention, and recall on the MMSE [17,40]. The present study observed no statistically significant differences in MMSE total score. In future studies, it would be helpful to target more patients and use cognitive assessments with reliable subdomain variables.
This study has several limitations. First, the stroke patients who participated in this study received conventional rehabilitation for 4 weeks between the baseline and the follow-up evaluations. Therefore, there was the possibility of bias introduced by the treatment intervention. However, cognitive and neglect treatments were administered similarly as per the patient’s functional level; therefore, the results are not expected to have a significant effect. Second, the number of patients was small and long-term prognosis evaluation is needed. Third, further study is needed to assess the relationship between elevated LBT grade and prognosis. In this study, the LBT grades were analyzed based on increase or maintenance and grade decrease. The grade change from 2 to 1 and from 3 to 1 can differ; however, a small number of samples made it difficult to finely classify the groups. Future studies should enroll more patients and perform comparisons among patients with different degrees of HSN severity.
In conclusion, the LBT is one of the most commonly used testing tools in HSN patients. This study observed a significant association between LBT performance time and HSN prognosis. In addition, the baseline MVPT-3 score can be considered in predicting prognosis. The measurement of LBT performance time is simple and can be used to predict the prognosis of HSN patients.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by a grant from the Korea Health Technology R&D Project through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute (KHIDI), funded by the Ministry of Health & Welfare, Republic of Korea (No. HI16C1559).
REFERENCES
1. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Coslett HB, Whelan H, Watson RT. Directional hypokinesia: prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect. Neurology. 1985; 35:855–9.
3. Bisiach E, Perani D, Vallar G, Berti A. Unilateral neglect: personal and extra-personal. Neuropsychologia. 1986; 24:759–67.
4. Stone SP, Wilson B, Wroot A, Halligan PW, Lange LS, Marshall JC, et al. The assessment of visuo-spatial neglect after acute stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1991; 54:345–50.
5. Denes G, Semenza C, Stoppa E, Lis A. Unilateral spatial neglect and recovery from hemiplegia: a follow-up study. Brain. 1982; 105:543–52.
6. Jehkonen M, Ahonen JP, Dastidar P, Koivisto AM, Laippala P, Vilkki J, et al. Visual neglect as a predictor of functional outcome one year after stroke. Acta Neurol Scand. 2000; 101:195–201.
7. Appelros P, Karlsson GM, Seiger A, Nydevik I. Prognosis for patients with neglect and anosognosia with special reference to cognitive impairment. J Rehabil Med. 2003; 35:254–8.
8. Azouvi P, Samuel C, Louis-Dreyfus A, Bernati T, Bartolomeo P, Beis JM, et al. Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002; 73:160–6.
9. Schenkenberg T, Bradford DC, Ajax ET. Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology. 1980; 30:509–17.
10. Ishiai S, Sugishita M, Ichikawa T, Gono S, Watabiki S. Clock-drawing test and unilateral spatial neglect. Neurology. 1993; 43:106–10.
13. Bailey MJ, Riddoch MJ, Crome P. Evaluation of a test battery for hemineglect in elderly stroke patients for use by therapists in clinical practice. NeuroRehabilitation. 2000; 14:139–50.
14. Van Deusen J. Normative data for ninety-three elderly persons on the Schenkenberg line bisection test. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr. 1985; 3:49–54.
15. Farne A, Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro M, Frassinetti F, Whyte J, Veramonti T, et al. Patterns of spontaneous recovery of neglect and associated disorders in acute right braindamaged patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004; 75:1401–10.
16. Halligan PW, Fink GR, Marshall JC, Vallar G. Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003; 7:125–33.
17. Lee BH, Kim EJ, Ku BD, Choi KM, Seo SW, Kim GM, et al. Cognitive impairments in patients with hemispatial neglect from acute right hemisphere stroke. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008; 21:73–6.
18. Yi YG, Chun MH, Do KH, Sung EJ, Kwon YG, Kim DY. The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on neglect syndrome in stroke patients. Ann Rehabil Med. 2016; 40:223–9.
19. Weinberg J, Diller L, Gerstman L, Schulman P. Digit span in right and left hemiplegics. J Clin Psychol. 1972; 28:361.
20. Robertson I. Anomalies in the laterality of omissions in unilateral left visual neglect: implications for an attentional theory of neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1989; 27:157–65.
21. Stuss DT, Stethem LL, Hugenholtz H, Picton T, Pivik J, Richard MT. Reaction time after head injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1989; 52:742–8.
22. Paus T, Zatorre RJ, Hofle N, Caramanos Z, Gotman J, Petrides M, et al. Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997; 9:392–408.
23. Brown GT, Rodger S, Davis A. Motor-free visual perception test—revised: an overview and critique. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2003; 66:159–67.
24. Brown T. An examination of the construct validity of the Motor-Free Visual Perceptual Test—Third Edition (MVPT-3) using Rasch analysis with adult participants. OTJR. 2001; 31:73–80.
25. Kang Y, Na DL, Hahn S. A validity study on the Korean mini-mental state examination (K-MMSE) in dementia patients. J Korean Neurol Assoc. 1997; 15:300–8.
26. Mesulam MM. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999; 354:1325–46.
27. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011; 34:569–99.
28. Whyte J. Attention and arousal: basic science aspects. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1992; 73:940–9.
29. Jang WH. Status of occupational therapists on unilateral neglect test tools usage and symptom classification. J Korean Phys Ther. 2017; 29:271–5.
30. Cha YJ, Kim K. The comparison of plantar pressure distribution regarding the extent of hemineglect in adult hemiplegia. J Korean Soc Phys Med. 2010; 5:43–51.
32. Rabuffetti M, Farina E, Alberoni M, Pellegatta D, Appollonio I, Affanni P, et al. Spatio-temporal features of visual exploration in unilaterally brain-damaged subjects with or without neglect: results from a touchscreen test. PLoS One. 2012; 7:e31511.
33. Paolucci S, Antonucci G, Grasso MG, Pizzamiglio L. The role of unilateral spatial neglect in rehabilitation of right brain-damaged ischemic stroke patients: a matched comparison. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001; 82:743–9.
34. Jeong EH, Kim BR, Lee J. Relationship between comorbid cognitive impairment and functional outcomes in stroke patients with spatial neglect. Brain Neurorehabil. 2016; 9:37–47.
35. Hjaltason H, Tegner R, Tham K, Levander M, Ericson K. Sustained attention and awareness of disability in chronic neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1996; 34:1229–33.
36. Mercier L, Hebert R, Gauthier L. Motor Free Visual Perceptual Test: impact of vertical answer cards position on performance of adults with hemispatial visual neglect. OTJR. 1995; 15:223–36.
37. Mazer BL, Korner-Bitensky NA, Sofer S. Predicting ability to drive after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1998; 79:743–50.
38. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975; 12:189–98.
Table 1.
Variable | Total (n=26) | Improved groupa) (n=13) | Non-improved groupa) (n=13) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (yr) | 60.4±10.5 | 57.6±11.2 | 63.2±9.4 | 0.15 |
Sex | 0.67 | |||
Male | 18 | 8 | 10 | |
Female | 8 | 5 | 3 | |
Etiology | 0.43 | |||
Ischemic stroke | 15 | 6 | 9 | |
Hemorrhagic stroke | 11 | 7 | 4 | |
Onset to baseline evaluation (day) | 41.8±87.0 | 56.6±122.2 | 27.0±18.6 | 0.55 |
Side of brain lesion | 0.44 | |||
Right | 19 | 10 | 9 | |
Left | 3 | 2 | 1 | |
Bilateral | 4 | 1 | 3 | |
Location of brain lesion | 0.37 | |||
Frontal | 13 | 6 | 7 | |
Parietal | 9 | 3 | 6 | |
Temporal | 5 | 2 | 3 | |
Othersb) | 13 | 8 | 5 | |
Baseline K-MBI score | 50.4±22.8 | 50.0±23.8 | 50.8±22.7 | 1.00 |
Received rTMS | 5 | 2 | 3 |
Table 2.
Variable | Improved group (n=13) | Non-improved group (n=13) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
LBT grade | |||
Baseline evaluation | 0.63 | ||
Grade 0 (normal) | 0 | 0 | |
Grade 1 (mild) | 9 | 10 | |
Grade 2 (moderate) | 1 | 1 | |
Grade 3 (severe) | 3 | 2 | |
After 4 weeks evaluation | 0.00** | ||
Grade 0 (normal) | 10 | 0 | |
Grade 1 (mild) | 2 | 8 | |
Grade 2 (moderate) | 0 | 2 | |
Grade 3 (severe) | 0 | 3 | |
LBT time (sec) | |||
Baseline LBT time | 61.45±25.12 | 101.15±58.57 | 0.03* |
ΔLBT time | 10.85±16.94 | 30.49±65.30 | 0.58 |
MVPT-3 | |||
Baseline MVPT-3 score | 41.00±11.14 | 28.46±17.60 | 0.20 |
ΔMVPT-3 score | 6.69±6.01 | 11.92±12.95 | 0.45 |
K-MMSE | |||
Baseline K-MMSE score | 23.00±4.69 | 19.69±5.14 | 0.15 |
ΔK-MMSE score | 3.77±2.98 | 5.62±2.53 | 0.07 |
Table 3.
Variables | OR | 95% CI | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
Age (yr) | 0.95 | 0.82–1.10 | 0.48 |
Sex, male | 48.68 | 1.20–1,967.42 | 0.04* |
Onset to baseline evaluation (day) | 1.11 | 1.00–1.24 | 0.06 |
Side of brain lesion, right | 1.95 | 0.01–414.86 | 0.81 |
Location of brain lesion | |||
Frontal | 40.97 | 0.22–7,704.45 | 0.16 |
Parietal | 0.00 | 0.00–1.71 | 0.07 |
Temporal | 212.76 | 0.11–400,574.19 | 0.16 |
Baseline LBT time (sec) | 0.95 | 0.90–1.00 | 0.04* |
Baseline MVPT-3 score | 1.20 | 1.01–1.43 | 0.04* |
Baseline K-MMSE score | 1.36 | 0.74–2.48 | 0.32 |