Abstract
BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVES
A cross-sectional study was undertaken to evaluate fluid intake and hydration status in association with cognitive function among 230 adolescents (10–14 years of age) in Petaling Perdana, Selangor, Malaysia.
SUBJECTS/METHODS
Urine color was used to measure hydration status, while fluid intake was assessed using the 15-item beverage intake questionnaire. Cognitive function was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition.
RESULTS
More than half of the adolescents were mildly or moderately dehydrated (59.6%) and only one-third (33.0%) were well hydrated. Among the daily fluid types, intakes of soft drinks (r = −0.180; P = 0.006), sweetened tea (r = −0.184; P = 0.005) and total sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) (r = −0.199; P = 0.002) were negatively correlated with cognitive function. In terms of hydration status, cognitive function score was significantly higher (F-ratio = 4.102; P = 0.018) among hydrated adolescents (100.38 ± 12.01) than in dehydrated (92.00 ± 13.63) counterparts. Hierarchical multiple linear regression analysis, after adjusting for socio-demographic factors, showed that soft drinks (β = −0.009; P < 0.05) and sweetened tea (β = −0.019; P < 0.05) negatively predicted cognitive function (ΔR2 = 0.044). When further control for sources of fluid, hydration status (β = −2.839; P < 0.05) was shown to negatively predict cognitive function (ΔR2 = 0.021). The above variables contributed 20.1% of the variance in cognitive function.
CONCLUSIONS
The results highlight the links between fluid intake (soft drinks, sweetened tea, total SSBs) and hydration status with cognitive function in adolescents. Interventions aimed at decreasing the consumption of SSBs and increasing hydration status through healthy fluid choices, such as water, could improve cognitive performance in adolescents.
Water, as a source of hydration, is essential for the physical and physiological functions of the human body [1]. However, previous evidence has shown that adolescents have an insufficient level of water intake, possibly due to a shift from consuming plain water to sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) as their primary source of fluid and hydration [2]. For instance, data for 8,109 adolescents (10–18 years old) retrieved from 13 cross-sectional surveys showed that more than 50% of the whole adolescent population were at risk of inadequate water intake [3]. Similarly, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey conducted among American children and adolescents aged 6–19 years old reported that 54.5% of that cohort had inadequate hydration [4].
Although it is known that proper hydration is important for human homeostasis and crucial for survival, a relationship between hydration status and brain function, as well as its role in cognitive performance, was only described recently. In particular, the effect of dehydration on cognitive function was shown to be relevant among individuals with poor fluid regulation, such as children, as they are at risk of voluntary dehydration [5]. A study conducted among 168 children aged 9–11 years showed a negative correlation between dehydration and short-term memory, indicating the importance and benefits of drinking water for hydration [6]. Similarly, another study conducted among 125 children with a mean age of 10.98 ± 0.38 years reported that the more water consumed by the children, the better they performed in concentration and memory tests [7]. To the best of our knowledge, only one study has reported on the effects of dehydration on cognitive function in adolescents, and the authors reported adverse effects of dehydration on executive functions such as planning and visuospatial processing [8].
Consumption of SSBs as a source of fluid and hydration is apparent among children and adolescents. Increases in SSB consumption has been observed in both developed and developing countries [910], possibly due to the widespread availability of cheap SSBs or soft drinks. Although the consequences of excessive SSB consumption are important in the development of childhood obesity [1112] and have been associated with increased cardiometabolic risks [9], less has been reported on the association of sugar consumption with cognition, behavior, and mental health [13]. A meta-analysis of sixteen intervention studies that examined the association between sugar and cognition revealed that the relationship was ambiguous [14].
Despite the importance of adequate hydration on health, there is a lack of published studies reporting the hydration status and fluid sources of adolescents in Malaysia [15], and there are no reports on the associations of hydration and fluid source with cognitive function. In addition, little has been reported on SSB consumption and cognitive function in adolescents. Most past studies were conducted using clinical or elderly cohorts [1617]. Hence, there was a need to determine the associations between fluid intake, hydration status, and cognitive function in adolescents. Therefore, this study was undertaken to examine the associations between fluid intake, hydration status, and cognitive function in adolescents in Petaling Perdana, Selangor, Malaysia.
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Research and Scholarly Activities of the Faculty of Applied Sciences, UCSI University (Proj-FAS-EC-16-001). Approval to conduct the study was obtained from the Ministry of Education, Malaysia (KPMSP.600-3/2/3JLD 27). Prior to data collection, authorization was obtained from participating primary school principals. Respondents and their parents were informed about the purpose of the study through information sheets distributed by their school teachers. Signed consent forms were obtained from the respondents and their parents prior to data collection.
This cross-sectional study was conducted among primary and secondary school children aged 10–14 years in the education district of Petaling Perdana, in the state of Selangor, Malaysia. A multi-stage sampling method was applied. Stratification by education was applied according to primary and secondary education levels. From a list of 57 schools that fulfilled the inclusion criteria of being multi-ethnic and co-educational, 2 national primary and 3 national secondary schools were randomly selected. From each school, one class from each year (primary 4 & 5; secondary 1 & 2) were selected randomly and invited to participate in this study. Parental information sheets and consent forms were distributed to the selected students via their teachers. Out of 500 child-parent pairs that were invited to participate, 230 child-parent pairs participated in and completed the study (response rate 46.0%).
To assess the socio-demographic background of the parent-child pairs, the child's date of birth, child's age, parent's sex, parent's ethnicity, parent's highest educational level, parent's occupation, and household monthly income information were gathered.
Habitual beverage intakes of children were assessed using the 15-item beverage intake questionnaire [16]. The responses provided estimates of mean daily intakes of water, all beverages, and SSBs. Questionnaire instructions with examples on how to answer the questions were provided by the researchers to the adolescents. The questionnaire included questions on twelve beverage categories: water, 100% fruit juice, sweetened juice beverage or drink, whole milk, low-fat milk, soft drinks, diet soft drinks, sweetened tea, tea or coffee with sugar and/or creamer, tea or coffee black without sugar and/or creamer, energy/sports drinks, and malted drinks, which are popular among Malaysians [17]. Alcoholic beverages and reduced-fat milk were excluded as alcoholic beverages are forbidden for Muslim adolescents and reduced-fat milk is not commonly available in Malaysia. Available responses included “how often” ranging from “never or less than once per week” to up “3+ times per day” and “how much” ranging from “less than ¾ cup” to “more than 2½ cups”. The total intake per week was divided by 7 to obtain the daily mean intake for each beverage. The SSBs category included 100% fruit juice, sweetened juice beverage, malt drinks, soft drinks, diet cola, sweetened tea, and tea/coffee with sugar and/or creamer. The daily mean intake of each beverage within the SSB category was summed to obtain the total SSB intake, whereas the daily mean intake of each type of fluid was summed to obtain the total daily fluid intake. The total daily fluid was then compared with the daily fluid intake requirements presented as Recommended Nutrient Intakes (RNIs) in the Malaysian Dietary Guidelines for Children and Adolescents [18].
A urine color chart [19] was used to assess the hydration status of the subjects. The urine color chart consisted of 8 color scales ranging from pale yellow or straw-colored (1) to brownish green (8). Urine colors 1 to 3 indicated “well-hydrated”, 4 indicated “normally hydrated or slightly dehydrated”, 5 and 6 indicated “dehydrated”, and 7 and 8 indicated “extremely dehydrated” [19]. Once the subjects were at school, subjects were provided with urine sample containers to collect first-morning urine. A total of 10 mL of urine was collected to represent the first-morning urine sample. Once the urine was collected, the urine color was determined by the same researcher.
The cognitive function of the subjects was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV), which is an appropriate test for children aged 6 to 16 years. To reduce test fatigue and burden of the respondents, only the similarities, digit span, block design, and coding subtests were used in this study. For each of the subtests, the raw scores were converted into scaled scores that were adjusted for age [20]. A composite score was derived from the 4 scaled subset scores and was converted into a cognitive function score using a conversion table provided by the provider of WISC-IV short forms [20]. The internal consistency of the cognitive function scores in the current study was acceptable (α = 0.71). Before use, the original subtests were translated into Malay and back-translated to English by 2 qualified psychologists who were proficient in both languages. The face validity of the items was then examined by a qualified and registered clinical psychologist. Before administration of the test, the researcher underwent relevant training by a qualified clinical psychologist. Test administration was conducted in a quiet room using uniform procedures.
Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 22.0; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Independent sample t-tests were used to determine differences between means of daily fluid intake, hydration status, and cognitive function of male and female subjects. One-way analysis of variance was used to compare differences in cognitive function across fathers' and mothers' educational level and monthly household income categories. Pearson correlation coefficients were used to determine the correlations between continuous variables. Hierarchical multiple linear regression was performed to test the contribution of individual variables to cognitive function. To determine the differential contribution of each block of variables toward the prediction of cognitive function, socio-demographic features (age, sex, ethnicity, education level, household income) were entered in the first block. Fluid intake types (water, 100% fruit juice, sweetened juice beverage, whole milk, low-fat/fat-free milk, malted drink, soft drinks, diet cola, sweetened tea, tea/coffee without sugar and/or creamer, tea/coffee without sugar and/or creamer, energy/sports drink) were used in the second block, and hydration status was used in the last block. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.
A total of 230 adolescents (37% male and 63% female; mean age 12.50 ± 1.61 years) participated in this study (Table 1). More than half (60.8%) of the adolescents were Malay, with the remaining being Chinese (19.6%) and Indian (19.6%). The majority of parents had at least a secondary school education (84.7% fathers and 70.8% mothers), and most (53.4%) had household incomes within the RM1,500 to RM5,999 range.
Daily fluid intake of the respondents is summarized in Table 2. Water was the main source of fluid intake (877.00 ± 492.08 mL/day) followed by malted drinks (221.88 ± 272.38 mL/day) and sweetened juice beverages (149.73 ± 200.14 mL/day). The least consumed beverages were low-fat milk (65.39 ± 141.62 mL/day), tea/coffee without sugar and/or creamer (30.92 ± 91.04 mL/day), and diet soft drinks (12.11 ± 42.89 mL/day). The mean total daily intake of SSBs of the male subjects (970.52 ± 738.78 mL/day) was significantly higher than that of females (673.54 ± 543.86 mL/day) (t = −3.088; P = 0.002). Similarly, the mean total daily fluid intake of males (2,149.26 ± 1,070.17 mL/day) was significantly higher than that of females (1,680 ± 724.61 mL/day) (t = −3.674; P = 0.001). When comparing total daily fluid intake with the RNI, 29.6% (36.5% males; 25.5% females) of the adolescents achieved the daily RNI of 2,000–2,200 mL of fluid [18]. No significant difference in RNI achievement was detected between the male and female subjects.
Hydration status and cognitive function of the respondents are summarized in Table 3. According to the urine color chart, more than half of the adolescents were mildly or moderately dehydrated (59.6%) or dehydrated (7.4%), while only one-third (33.0%) of the adolescents were well hydrated. There was no significant difference in hydration status between the male and female subjects. More than half (59.3%) of the respondents had an average cognitive function score, while 22.2% and 18.5% had below- or above-average cognitive function, respectively. No significant difference between average cognitive function scores of males and females was detected.
Bivariate analysis results are summarized in Table 4. The analysis revealed that the cognitive function score was significantly higher (F-ratio = 5.087; P = 0.002) among adolescents of fathers with tertiary education level (102.75 ± 12.46) compared to adolescents of fathers with primary (96.20 ± 8.64) or secondary (95.50 ± 10.27) education, but there was no difference among the various educational levels of mothers. The cognitive function score among adolescents from households with a monthly income of RM1,500 (USD360) and above (98.54 ± 11.30) was significantly higher (F-ratio = 6.512; P < 0.001) than the cognitive function score of adolescents from households with a mean monthly income of less than RM1,500 (83.00 ± 8.58). However, age (r = 0.029; P = 0.661), sex (t = −0.615; P = 0.539), and mother's educational level (F-ratio = 0.977; P = 0.421) were not significantly associated with cognitive function score. Regarding daily fluid intake, soft drinks (r = −0.180; P = 0.006), sweetened tea (r = −0.184; P = 0.005), and total SSBs (r = −0.199; P = 0.002) intakes were negatively correlated with cognitive function score in adolescents, but no significant relationship was detected for the other beverage categories. Concerning hydration status, the mean cognitive function score was significantly higher (F-ratio = 4.102; P = 0.018) among hydrated adolescents (100.38 ± 12.01) than in dehydrated adolescents (92.00 ± 13.63).
Table 5 provides a summary of the results of the hierarchical multiple linear regression model with cognitive function as the outcome. The addition of fluid intake and hydration status increased the overall fit of the model. Fathers' education (β = 0.745; P < 0.05) and household income (β = 2.642; P < 0.001) were significant positive predictors of cognitive function of adolescents (ΔR2 = 0.136). After controlling for socio-demographic variables, soft drinks (β = −0.009; P < 0.05) and sweetened tea (β = −0.019; P < 0.05) were significant negative predictors of cognitive function (ΔR2 = 0.044). When further controlled for sources of fluid, hydration status (β = −2.839; P < 0.05) was a significant negative predictor of cognitive function of adolescents (ΔR2 = 0.021). The variables explained 20.1% of the variances in cognitive function of adolescents (R2 = 0.201).
The results show that less than one-third (29.6%) of the adolescents in this study meet the RNI of 2,000–2,200 mL of fluid daily [21], and that result is similar to those in previous studies [15]. The adolescents' mean total daily fluid intake in the current study was much lower than those in Indonesia [22] and Latin America [23] but higher than that in European adolescents [24] Those results were consistent with the hydration status results for the adolescents, which showed that more than half (67.0%) of the adolescents were inadequately hydrated which was similar to the results of a study in Egypt [25] and much higher than that reported in a study in the United States [4].
The pattern of beverage consumption observed in this study was similar to those in past studies in which plain water was the main source of fluid [1526]. In this study, water intake was followed by that for malted drinks, a fluid source that is prevalent among Malaysian children [17]. However, in other countries [26], milk consumption is more prevalent, indicating that the consumption of milk is notably lower among Malaysian adolescents. Our results also showed that a significant proportion of the fluid intake by Malaysian adolescents was from SSBs, and that proportion was much higher than those in Indonesia [22] and Latin America [23], indicating that SSBs are a frequent replacement of plain water as a significant fluid source among Malay adolescents. This high level of SSB consumption has raised concerns due to the link between SSBs and obesity, type 2 diabetes, and cardiometabolic risks [911].
The current study revealed that consumption of soft drinks, sweetened tea, and total SSBs were negatively correlated with the cognitive function level of the adolescents. When soft drinks and sweetened tea were included in our hierarchical model, they remained as negative predictors of cognitive function in adolescents. To date, few studies have reported on the effects of SSBs on cognition. One meta-analysis of 16 intervention studies that examined the association between sugar and cognition has been reported, and the study concluded that the relationship was ambiguous [14]. Nevertheless, results in some animal studies support the suggestion that excess sugar intake may adversely influence cognitive performance and lead to poor executive function [1327]. Another study that examined the association of sugar intake with cognition in children showed that sugar consumption, especially that in SSBs, during childhood was inversely associated with verbal intelligence scores at mid-childhood [28]. One explanation for this inverse relationship is that sugar in such beverages can damage the hippocampus, which in turn can impair spatial learning and memory, as has been reported in a previous animal study [29]. Intake of SSBs can also result in lower cognitive performance through metabolic-related complications such as elevated blood sugar, insulin resistance [30], and inflammation [31].
Similar to the results in past studies, when hydration status was examined, adolescents who were well hydrated had a better cognitive performance level than that in those who were dehydrated [67]. In the present study, this relationship remained after controlling for socio-demographic factors and the consumption of soft drinks and sweetened tea in the hierarchical model. A possible explanation for a reduced level of cognition due to dehydration is that dehydration can alter the level of electrolytes in the body. Changes in electrolyte levels associated with dehydration can alter brain activity and neurotransmission system performance, leading to alteration of cognitive function [32]. In addition, dehydration can cause changes in blood-brain barrier permeability, leading to a decrease in the amount of blood flow in areas of the brain, among which some are responsible for specific cognitive function [33].
To our knowledge, this is the first study that associated SSB consumption and hydration status with cognitive function among Malaysian adolescents. However, the variables measured were only able to explain 20.1% of the variance in cognitive function. Other factors related to cognitive function such as sleep habits [34], physical activity [3536], and diet [36] of the adolescents were not examined in this study. Examining these additional correlates of cognitive function may have increased the level of variance in the adolescents' cognitive function that was explained by the model.
Several limitations of the present study should be considered. Causal inferences are not possible due to the cross-sectional nature of this study. Furthermore, as the respondents were only recruited from one geographical area in Malaysia, generalizability to all adolescents in the country was not possible. Additionally, there are possible inaccuracies in the answers provided by the subjects due to the effects of response bias and social desirability. Despite these limitations, the current study did improve on the level of information provided by previous research. As little has been reported on the relationship of SSBs consumption and hydration status with cognitive function, the current study expands the level of knowledge presented in earlier studies, particularly in an adolescent Malaysian context.
In conclusion, some of the highlights of the study results include the revelation of links between fluid intake (soft drinks, sweetened tea, total SSBs) and hydration status with cognitive function in adolescents. Although these factors may not entirely explain alterations in the cognitive performance of adolescents, some fluid sources, such as SSBs, and hydration status do have important roles in altering cognitive performance in adolescents. With the increasing consumption of SSBs among adolescents, an expansion of efforts to promote healthier fluid intake options to stay hydrated may help to prevent adverse effects on adolescents' cognition. Interventions aimed to decrease the consumption of SSBs and increase hydration status through healthier options, such as water, should be developed. Fluid-related programs aimed to improve cognitive performance of adolescents may also have bodyweight- and mood-related benefits.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank all the adolescents involved for their participation and cooperation and their parents for permission and support during this study. We are also grateful to the school principals and teachers as well as the Ministry of Education for their cooperation and assistance.
Notes
Funding: This project was funded by the UCSI University Pioneer Scientist Incentive Fund (PSIF) (Proj-In-FAS-035).
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization: Tung SEH, Ch'ng YZ, Karnan TV, Chong PN, Zubaidah JO, Chin YS.
Data curation: Tung SEH, Ch'ng YZ, Karnan TV, Chong PN, Zubaidah JO, Chin YS.
Formal analysis: Tung SEH.
Funding acquisition: Tung SEH, Chong PN.
Investigation: Tung SEH.
Methodology: Tung SEH.
Supervision: Tung SEH.
Writing - original draft: Tung SEH.
Writing - review & editing: Chin YS, Chong PN, Zubaidah JO.
References
1. D'Anci KE, Constant F, Rosenberg IH. Hydration and cognitive function in children. Nutr Rev. 2006; 64:457–464. PMID: 17063927.
2. Muckelbauer R, Barbosa CL, Mittag T, Burkhardt K, Mikelaishvili N, Müller-Nordhorn J. Association between water consumption and body weight outcomes in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2014; 22:2462–2475. PMID: 25401930.
3. Iglesia I, Guelinckx I, De Miguel-Etayo PM, González-Gil EM, Salas-Salvadó J, Kavouras SA, Gandy J, Martínez H, Bardosono S, Abdollahi M, Nasseri E, Jarosz A, Ma G, Carmuega E, Thiébaut I, Moreno LA. Total fluid intake of children and adolescents: cross-sectional surveys in 13 countries worldwide. Eur J Nutr. 2015; 54(Suppl 2):57–67. PMID: 26081646.
4. Kenney EL, Long MW, Cradock AL, Gortmaker SL. Prevalence of inadequate hydration among US children and disparities by gender and race/ethnicity: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2009–2012. Am J Public Health. 2015; 105:e113–8. PMID: 26066941.
5. Masento NA, Golightly M, Field DT, Butler LT, van Reekum CM. Effects of hydration status on cognitive performance and mood. Br J Nutr. 2014; 111:1841–1852. PMID: 24480458.
6. Fadda R, Rapinett G, Grathwohl D, Parisi M, Fanari R, Calò CM, Schmitt J. Effects of drinking supplementary water at school on cognitive performance in children. Appetite. 2012; 59:730–737. PMID: 22841529.
7. Fuchs T, Lührmann P, Simpson F, Dohnke B. Fluid intake and cognitive performance: should schoolchildren drink during lessons? J Sch Health. 2016; 86:407–413. PMID: 27122140.
8. Kempton MJ, Ettinger U, Foster R, Williams SC, Calvert GA, Hampshire A, Zelaya FO, O'Gorman RL, McMorris T, Owen AM, Smith MS. Dehydration affects brain structure and function in healthy adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011; 32:71–79. PMID: 20336685.
9. Loh DA, Moy FM, Zaharan NL, Jalaludin MY, Mohamed Z. Sugar-sweetened beverage intake and its associations with cardiometabolic risks among adolescents. Pediatr Obes. 2017; 12:e1–5. PMID: 26843446.
10. Ha K, Chung S, Lee HS, Kim CI, Joung H, Paik HY, Song Y. Association of dietary sugars and sugar-sweetened beverage intake with obesity in Korean children and adolescents. Nutrients. 2016; 8:31.
11. Luger M, Lafontan M, Bes-Rastrollo M, Winzer E, Yumuk V, Farpour-Lambert N. Sugar-sweetened beverages and weight gain in children and adults: a systematic review from 2013 to 2015 and a comparison with previous studies. Obes Facts. 2017; 10:674–693. PMID: 29237159.
12. Hu FB. Resolved: there is sufficient scientific evidence that decreasing sugar-sweetened beverage consumption will reduce the prevalence of obesity and obesity-related diseases. Obes Rev. 2013; 14:606–619. PMID: 23763695.
13. Reichelt AC, Killcross S, Hambly LD, Morris MJ, Westbrook RF. Impact of adolescent sucrose access on cognitive control, recognition memory, and parvalbumin immunoreactivity. Learn Mem. 2015; 22:215–224. PMID: 25776039.
14. Wolraich ML, Wilson DB, White JW. The effect of sugar on behavior or cognition in children. A meta-analysis. JAMA. 1995; 274:1617–1621. PMID: 7474248.
15. Kaur S, Tung S, Maykanathan D, Lim YY. The association of the hydration status and parental knowledge on fluid consumption with children's weight status in Malaysia. Sri Lanka J Child Health. 2017; 46:222–229.
16. Hedrick VE, Comber DL, Estabrooks PA, Savla J, Davy BM. The beverage intake questionnaire: determining initial validity and reliability. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010; 110:1227–1232. PMID: 20656099.
17. Jan Mohamed JM, Loy SL, Mohd Taib MN, Karim NA, Tan SY, Appukutty M, Razak NA, Thielecke F, Hopkins S, Ong MK, Ning C, Tee ES. Characteristics associated with the consumption of malted drinks among Malaysian primary school children: findings from the MyBreakfast study. BMC Public Health. 2015; 15:1322. PMID: 26718818.
18. National Coordinating Committee on Food and Nutrition (NCCFN), Ministry of Health Malaysia. RNI: recommended nutrient intakes for Malaysia [Internet]. Putrajaya: Ministry of Health Malaysia;2017. cited 2019 May 10. Available from: http://nutrition.moh.gov.my.
19. Armstrong LE, Soto JA, Hacker FJ, Casa DJ, Kavouras SA, Maresh CM. Urinary indices during dehydration, exercise, and rehydration. Int J Sport Nutr. 1998; 8:345–355. PMID: 9841955.
20. Sattler JM. Resource Guide to Accompany Assessment of Children: Cognitive Foundations. 5th ed. San Diego: J.M. Sattler;2008. p. 35–36.
21. National Coordinating Committee on Food and Nutrition (NCCFN), Ministry of Health Malaysia. Recommended Nutrient Intakes for Malaysia: a Report of the Technical Working Group on Nutritional Guidelines. Putrajaya: Ministry of Health Malaysia;2005.
22. Laksmi PW, Morin C, Gandy J, Moreno LA, Kavouras SA, Martinez H, Salas-Salvadó J, Guelinckx I. Fluid intake of children, adolescents and adults in Indonesia: results of the 2016 Liq.In7 national cross-sectional survey. Eur J Nutr. 2018; 57:89–100. PMID: 29923119.
23. Martinez H, Morin C, Gandy J, Carmuega E, Arredondo JL, Pimentel C, Moreno LA, Kavouras SA, Salas-Salvadó J, Guelinckx I. Fluid intake of Latin American adults: results of four 2016 Liq.In7 national cross-sectional surveys. Eur J Nutr. 2018; 57:65–75. PMID: 29858627.
24. Moreno LA, Iglesia-Altaba I, Santaliestra-Pasías AM. Fluid intake of European children and adolescents. Nutr Today. 2013; 48:S25–30.
25. Gouda Z, Zarea M, El-Hennawy U, Viltard M, Lepicard E, Hawili N, Constant F. Hydration deficit in 9- to 11-year-old Egyptian children. Glob Pediatr Health. 2015; 2:2333794X15611786.
26. Guelinckx I, Iglesia I, Bottin JH, De Miguel-Etayo P, González-Gil EM, Salas-Salvadó J, Kavouras SA, Gandy J, Martinez H, Bardosono S, Abdollahi M, Nasseri E, Jarosz A, Ma G, Carmuega E, Thiébaut I, Moreno LA. Intake of water and beverages of children and adolescents in 13 countries. Eur J Nutr. 2015; 54(Suppl 2):69–79. PMID: 26072216.
27. Ames SL, Kisbu-Sakarya Y, Reynolds KD, Boyle S, Cappelli C, Cox MG, Dust M, Grenard JL, Mackinnon DP, Stacy AW. Inhibitory control effects in adolescent binge eating and consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages and snacks. Appetite. 2014; 81:180–192. PMID: 24949566.
28. Cohen JF, Rifas-Shiman SL, Young J, Oken E. Associations of prenatal and child sugar intake with child cognition. Am J Prev Med. 2018; 54:727–735. PMID: 29674185.
29. Murray AM, Barzilay JI, Lovato JF, Williamson JD, Miller ME, Marcovina S, Launer LJ. Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) Substudy Investigators. Biomarkers of renal function and cognitive impairment in patients with diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2011; 34:1827–1832. PMID: 21715519.
30. Messier C. Impact of impaired glucose tolerance and type 2 diabetes on cognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2005; 26(Suppl 1):26–30. PMID: 16236384.
31. Wärnberg J, Gomez-Martinez S, Romeo J, Díaz LE, Marcos A. Nutrition, inflammation, and cognitive function. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009; 1153:164–175. PMID: 19236339.
32. Wilson MM, Morley JE. Impaired cognitive function and mental performance in mild dehydration. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2003; 57(Suppl 2):S24–S29. PMID: 14681710.
33. Maughan RJ, Shirreffs SM, Watson P. Exercise, heat, hydration and the brain. J Am Coll Nutr. 2007; 26:604S–612S. PMID: 17921473.
34. de Bruin EJ, van Run C, Staaks J, Meijer AM. Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning of adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2017; 32:45–57. PMID: 27039223.
35. Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2003; 15:243–256.
36. Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond). 2014; 38:494–506. PMID: 23913029.