Abstract
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to explore Inattentional Blindness (IB) as experienced by hospital nurses.
Methods
Data were collected from August 3 to October 30, 2017 through focus group interviews with 24 nurses working in a university hospital in Seoul, Korea. Four focus group interviews were conducted and all interviews were recorded. Data were analyzed through qualitative content analysis.
Results
Three categories and seven subcategories were identified from 96 units of analysis, as follows: 1) occurrence of IB (preoccupation with specific factors, pressure from external factors, unexpected event); 2) triggers of IB (distraction, low expectation of error); and 3) influences due to exposure to IB (continuous self-surveillance, perceived need for thinking expansion).
Conclusion
The findings of this study show that nurses experienced IB directly and indirectly while performing their duties. IB occurred especially in common nursing situations, such as health assessment and medication administration. The results also suggest that it is necessary to develop strategies to prevent IB in the clinical setting, which can be useful not only to ensure patient safety but also to help patients regain their health.
REFERENCES
1. Scanlon MC, Karsh BT. Value of human factors to medication and patient safety in the intensive care unit. Critical Care Medicine. 2010; 38(6 Suppl):S90–6. https://doi.org/10.1097/CCM.0b013e3181dd8de2.
2. McKenna L, Missen K, Cooper S, Bogossian F, Bucknall T, Cant R. Situation awareness in undergraduate nursing students managing simulated patient deterioration. Nurse Education Today. 2014; 34(6):e27–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2013.12.013.
3. Paparella S. How did I miss that? The safety challenges of inattentional blindness. Journal of Emergency Nursing. 2013; 39(4):358–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jen.2013.03.005.
4. Westbrook JI, Raban MZ, Walter SR, Douglas H. Task errors by emergency physicians are associated with interruptions, multi-tasking, fatigue and working memory capacity: a prospective, direct observation study. BMJ Quality & Safety. 2018; 27(8):655–63. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2017-007333.
5. Gladwell M. Blink: the guide to thinking without thinking. New York: Little, Brown and Company;2015. p. 23–108.
6. Chabris C, Simons D. The invisible gorilla: and other ways our intuitions deceive us. 1st ed.New York: Crown Publishing Group;2010. p. 13–215.
7. O'Regan JK. Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie. 1992; 46(3):461–88.
8. Simons DJ, Rensink RA. Change blindness: past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005; 9(1):16–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.006.
9. Carayon P, Xie A, Kianfar S. Human factors and ergonomics as a patient safety practice. BMJ Quality & Safety. 2014; 23(3):196–205. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2013-001812.
10. Simons DJ, Jensen MS. The effects of individual differences and task difficulty on inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009; 16(2):398–403. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.398.
11. Lum TE, Fairbanks RJ, Pennington EC, Zwemer FL. Profiles in patient safety: misplaced femoral line guidewire and multiple failures to detect the foreign body on chest radiography. Academic Emergency Medicine. 2005; 12(7):658–62. https://doi.org/10.1197/j.aem.2005.02.014.
12. Drew T, Võ ML, Wolfe JM. The invisible gorilla strikes again: sustained inattentional blindness in expert observers. Psychological Science. 2013; 24(9):1848–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613479386.
13. Memmert D. The effects of eye movements, age, and expertise on inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition. 2006; 15(3):620–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.01.001.
14. Greig PR, Higham H, Nobre AC. Failure to perceive clinical events: an under-recognised source of error. Resuscitation. 2014; 85(7):952–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resuscitation.2014.03.316.
15. Kreitz C, Furley P, Memmert D, Simons DJ. Inattentional blindness and individual differences in cognitive abilities. PLOS one. 2015; 10(8):e0134675. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134675.
16. Apfelbaum HL, Gambacorta C, Woods RL, Peli E. Inattentional blindness with the same scene at different scales. Ophthal-mic & Physiological Optics. 2010; 30(2):124–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-1313.2009.00702.x.
17. Simons DJ, Chabris CF. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception. 1999; 28(9):1059–74. https://doi.org/10.1068/p281059.
18. Jones A, Johnstone MJ. Inattentional blindness and failures to rescue the deteriorating patient in critical care, emergency and perioperative settings: four case scenarios. Australian Critical Care. 2017; 30(4):219–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aucc.2016.09.005.
19. Gosbee J. Handoffs and communication: the underappreciated roles of situational awareness and inattentional blindness. Clinical Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2010; 53(3):545–58. https://doi.org/10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181ec1ac7.
20. Morgan DL. Focus groups as qualitative research. 2nd ed.London: Sage Publication, Inc;1997. p. 31–71.
21. Elo S, Kyngäs H. The qualitative content analysis process. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2008; 62(1):107–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x.
22. Lincoln YS, Guba EG. Naturalistic inquiry. Newbery, Califor-nia: SAGE publications;1985. p. 221–381.
23. Potchen EJ. Measuring observer performance in chest radiology: some experiences. Journal of the American College of Radiology. 2006; 3(6):423–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jacr.2006.02.020.
24. Koivisto M, Hyönä J, Revonsuo A. The effects of eye movements, spatial attention, and stimulus features on inattentional blindness. Vision Research. 2004; 44(27):3211–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.026.
25. Calvillo DP, Jackson RE. Animacy, perceptual load, and inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2014; 21(3):670–5. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0543-8.
26. Drew T, Stothart C. Clarifying the role of target similarity, task relevance and feature-based suppression during sustained inattentional blindness. Journal of Vision. 2016; 16(15):e1–9. https://doi.org/10.1167/16.15.13.
27. Richards A, Hannon EM, Vitkovitch M. Distracted by dis-tractors: eye movements in a dynamic inattentional blindness task. Consciousness and Cognition. 2012; 21(1):170–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.013.
28. Richards A, Hannon EM, Derakshan N. Predicting and manip-ulating the incidence of inattentional blindness. Psychological Research. 2010; 74(6):513–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0273-8.
29. Mack A, Pappas Z, Silverman M, Gay R. What we see: inattention and the capture of attention by meaning. Consciousness and Cognition. 2002; 11(4):488–506. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00028-4.
30. Christofidis MJ, Hill A, Horswill MS, Watson MO. A human factors approach to observation chart design can trump health professionals' prior chart experience. Resuscitation. 2013; 84(5):657–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resuscitation.2012.09.023.
31. Karsh BT, Holden RJ, Alper SJ, Or CKL. A human factors en-gineering paradigm for patient safety: designing to support the performance of the healthcare professional. BMJ Quality & Safety. 2006; 15(Suppl 1):i59–65. https://doi.org/10.1136/qshc.2005.015974.