Abstract
Background and Purpose
Korean-speaking patients with a brain injury may show agraphia that differs from that of English-speaking patients due to the unique features of Hangul syllabic writing. Each grapheme in Hangul must be arranged from left to right and/or top to bottom within a square space to form a syllable, which requires greater visuospatial abilities than when writing the letters constituting an alphabetic writing system. Among the Hangul grapheme positions within a syllable, the position of a vowel is important because it determines the writing direction and the whole configuration in Korean syllabic writing. Due to the visuospatial characteristics of the Hangul vowel, individuals with early-onset Alzheimer's disease (EOAD) may experiences differences between the difficulties of writing Hangul vowels and consonants due to prominent visuospatial dysfunctions caused by parietal lesions.
Methods
Eighteen patients with EOAD and 18 age-and-education-matched healthy adults participated in this study. The participants were requested to listen to and write 30 monosyllabic characters that consisted of an initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant with a one-to-one phoneme-to-grapheme correspondence. We measured the writing time for each grapheme, the pause time between writing the initial consonant and the medial vowel (P1), and the pause time between writing the medial vowel and the final consonant (P2).
Results
All grapheme writing and pause times were significantly longer in the EOAD group than in the controls. P1 was also significantly longer than P2 in the EOAD group.
Conclusions
Patients with EOAD might require a higher judgment ability and longer processing time for determining the visuospatial grapheme position before writing medial vowels. This finding suggests that a longer pause time before writing medial vowels is an early marker of visuospatial dysfunction in patients with EOAD.
The Korean writing system, Hangul (Hangeul), consists of 24 characters with 10 vowels (known as “/mo-eum/”; ㅏ, ㅑ, ㅓ, ㅕ, ㅗ, ㅛ, ㅜ, ㅠ, ㅡ, ㅣ) and 14 consonants (known as “/ja-eum/”; ㄱ, ㄴ, ㄷ, ㄹ, ㅁ, ㅂ, ㅅ, ㅇ, ㅈ, ㅊ, ㅋ, ㅌ, ㅍ, ㅎ). All Hangul graphemes correspond to a single phoneme. Korean syllables typically include an onset, which is a consonant (/cho-seong/), followed by a vowel (/jung-seong/), which may be followed by another consonant (/jong-seong/). Therefore, each syllable contains at least one consonant grapheme and one vowel, plus the optional additional of a final consonant.
In terms of the arrangement of graphemes within a syllable, Hangul has unique visuospatial/constructional features. Unlike how graphemes are written horizontally and in tandem in alphabetic writing systems, each grapheme in Hangul must be arranged from left to right and/or top to bottom within a square space to form a syllable. Among the grapheme positions within a syllable, the position of a vowel is particularly important because it determines the writing direction and the whole configuration in Korean syllabic writing (Fig. 1).1 The writing is oriented vertically (i.e., top to bottom) if the vowel is positioned at the bottom of the initial consonant, while the writing is oriented horizontally (left to right) or has a mixed orientation (left to right and top to bottom) if the vowel is positioned on the right side of the initial consonant within a syllable. Therefore, determining the vowel position in Hangul syllabic writing might be more closely related to visuoconstructional functions compared to determining the positions of other graphemes in a syllable.
In general, the function of visuospatial construction is processed by the biparietal lobes.2 Patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) present with symptoms associated with temporoparietal damage in the early stage that are driven by the deposition of beta-amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the brain. In particular, hypometabolism in the parietal area is more severe in patients with early-onset Alzheimer's disease (EOAD) than in those with late-onset Alzheimer's disease (LOAD).3 A previous study of agraphia in Korean patients with EOAD found that these patients showed visuoconstructional writing errors (i.e., stroke omission, addition, and misconfiguration) as well as linguistic errors (i.e., grapheme substitution, addition, and omission) even in the early stages of the disease.4 Because of this visuospatial characteristic of Hangul vowels, patients with AD (and especially those with EOAD) who have parietal lesions may show difficulties in differentiating between vowels and consonants.
The main aim of the present study was to delineate the visuoconstructional value of Hangul writing and the vowel grapheme positions within a Hangul syllable. This was achieved by asking patients with EOAD to write 30 dictated monosyllabic stimuli (vertical and mixed orientations including the initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant) and measuring the elapsed time for writing. Due to the unique characteristics of the vowel grapheme positional value and the disease entity of EOAD, we hypothesized that the time delay before writing the vowel grapheme would be longer than that for the other graphemes within a syllable.
Eighteen patients diagnosed with EOAD were recruited from the Memory Disorder Clinic at the Department of Neurology, Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, Korea. The patients were aged 57.44±6.47 years mean±standard deviation) and comprised 5 men and 13 women. They had 11.61±3.53 years of formal education, and their score on the Clinical Dementia Rating scale was 1.08±0.46, while that for the Mini Mental State Examination was 17.00±4.08. The following inclusion criteria were applied: 1) >6 years of education, to ensure that they had no difficulty writing Hangul in the premorbid state; 2) right-handed speakers of the standard Korean language who reported no history of writing disturbances before the diagnosis; 3) no history of neurological motor impairments such as weakness, rigidity, or tremor; and 4) language abilities confirmed using the Korean version of the Western Aphasia Battery,5 with an aphasia quotient of 75.17±17.75 and a language quotient of 70.77±18.42.
In order to compare the writing performance, we recruited 18 age- and education-matched healthy adults (5 men and 13 women) who were caregivers from the same memory disorder clinics at our hospital. The age of these normal controls (NC) was 56.33±6.42 years and they had 12.17±3.73 years of formal education. None of the NC had a history of neurological, language, or psychiatric disorders.
The writing-to-dictation task consisted of 30 single-syllable characters that had a one-to-one phoneme-to-grapheme correspondence (Table 1). Korean syllabic characters comprise either the combination of an initial consonant, a medial vowel, and a final consonant or the combination of an initial consonant and a medial vowel; this study only employed the first type of characters. The stimuli were selected based on their configuration, frequency, and imageability. Participants with EOAD who have temporoparietal lesions may experience writing difficulty not only due to a linguistic disability but also cognitive deficits. We minimized the contribution of cognitive deficits to agraphia by selecting easier single-syllables. In accordance with previous studies of AD,46 words appearing in the Learner's Dictionary of Korean7 at frequencies above 30% were selected in order to make the task less complicated and thereby reduce the cognitive load. In terms of configuration, characters with up to four graphemes were selected in order to further reduce the complexity of the task. In terms of imageability, the same proportion of concrete (80%) and abstract (20%) words were included in each category.7
Participants were asked to write each character on the screen of a tablet PC (IBM ThinkPad X220T) using a stylus pen. A program using ‘Quarsar’ was devised to measure the total writing time, which consists of the grapheme writing times plus the pause times while writing. Before the Hangul writing test, the patients were instructed to trace simple horizontal and vertical lines (both 4 cm long) at right angles to each other on the computer screen, and the elapsed time for general hand movement for drawing was measured. After drawing these two simple lines, the patients were instructed to write each character as meticulously as possible on an 8×8 cm2 space, the margin of which was framed on the screen by a black 1-mm-thick line. Each of the Hangul stimuli was verbally presented to the participants with a word cue [e.g., “please write the syllable ‘남’ (/nam/) of ‘남자’ (/nam-ja/ means ‘man’)”], and they were asked to repeat the target stimuli verbally before writing it to ensure that there was no deficit in auditory input ability. No time limit was imposed. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Samsung Medical Center (IRB 2006-03-011).
To investigate the general performance of Hangul writing, we first counted the number of erroneous responses according to the each grapheme position (i.e., initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant). The minimum and maximum numbers of erroneous responses within each position were 0 and 30, respectively. We inspected these errors from both linguistic and visuoconstructional points of view. The criteria used for the inspection from the linguistic and visuoconstructional aspects were based on previous studies:46 the linguistic errors consisted of the omission, substitution, and addition of graphemes, and the visuoconstructional errors consisted of stroke omission, stroke addition, and misconfiguration. Two speech-language pathologists analyzed the writing performance of each participant based on these criteria. To determine the interobserver agreement, we randomly chose two participants (constituting about 10% of the total number) from the patient group. The rate of agreement between the two raters was 98%, and consensus was used to resolve cases of discrepancy.
We also measured the elapsed time for each writing response. Considering the mean response/reaction time across all responses (i.e., including errors) would mean that the estimated times would have included processes that are unlikely to be of interest, and so using response/reaction times only from correct responses is a more-representative measure of time because it eliminates the erroneous responses.8 Accordingly, we measured the elapsed time only for correct responses. As illustrated in Fig. 2, the total writing time was defined as the time that had elapsed from the moment the participant touched the screen to write the character to the moment the participant released the pen from the touchscreen when the character had been written. Subsequently, the total writing time consisted of three grapheme writing times (G1, G2, and G3 for the initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant, respectively) plus two pause times (P1 and P2). The grapheme writing time was the elapsed writing time for each grapheme, which was defined as the duration from the moment when the screen was first pressed to the moment when the pen was released from the touchscreen. The pause time corresponded to the duration between the end of writing one grapheme and beginning writing the next grapheme: P1 was the pause time between writing the initial consonant and the medial vowel, and P2 was the pause time between writing the medial vowel and the final consonant.
Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was employed to evaluate the effect of the group (NC and EOAD), the number of erroneous responses according to grapheme position (initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant), and interactions between the group and the number of erroneous responses. The between-group analysis revealed that the number of errors in all grapheme positions (initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant) was significantly higher in the EOAD group than in the NC group (p<0.001) (Table 2). There were significantly interactions between the group and the number of erroneous responses (p<0.01).
Within each group, multiple-comparisons analysis with Bonferroni adjustment was performed to compare the performance according to the grapheme position in the Hangul syllable. In terms of the number of errors among the grapheme positions in the Hangul syllable, the performance differed significantly in the patient group (p<0.001), with the number of errors being significantly higher for the medial vowel than for the initial or final consonant (p<0.05, with Bonferroni adjustment). Based on the criteria using in a previous study,4 the error patterns in the patient group were distributed as follows: 38.8% linguistic errors, 23.6% visuoconstructional errors, 18.8% mixed (linguistic plus visuoconstructional) errors, and 18.8% undetermined errors.
The elapsed time for drawing simple lines served as the control for motor speed, and this did not differ significantly between the NC and EOAD groups (p>0.05). In terms of the writing time, two-way ANOVA was employed to evaluate the effect of group (NC and EOAD), total writing time, and interactions between the group and the total writing time. The between-group analysis showed that all grapheme writing and pause times were significantly longer in the EOAD group than in the NC group (p<0.001) (Table 3). There were also significantly interactions between the group and the grapheme writing time (p<0.01).
Within each group, multiple-comparisons analyses with Bonferroni adjustment were performed to compare the performance according to the grapheme position in the Hangul syllable. The grapheme writing times (G1, G2, and G3) and pause times (P1 and P2) were analyzed separately. G1, G2, and G3 differed significantly in both the NC and EOAD groups (p<0.001). The multiple-comparisons analysis revealed that the grapheme writing time was significantly shorter for G3 than for G1 or G2 (p<0.05, with Bonferroni adjustment). Pause times P1 and P2 differed in the EOAD group, with P1 being significantly longer than P2 (p<0.01, with Bonferroni adjustment). No significant difference between P1 and P2 was detected in the NC group.
The first major finding of this study was that the difficulties experienced by the patients differed between writing vowels and consonants. Previous case studies of Italian and Turkish patients using alphabetic writing systems found that information about consonant and vowel representations might be separately handled.91011 One of these studies found a selective vowel deficit in writing in two patients,9 while the two other case studies found a writing disorder for consonants.1011 From a linguistic point of view, our results support the notion that the consonant and vowel status of graphemes might differentially affect the spelling process.
The mechanism underlying why the frequency of errors was higher for vowels than for consonants in our patients remains to be elucidated. Linguistically, consonants and vowels represent two different qualities of sounds: consonants are defined as sounds articulated by the temporary obstruction in the airstream that passes through the mouth, with distinctive contributions from each articulator structure such as the lip, alveolar, tongue, or velum; in contrast, vowels are produced without any obstruction of the air passage, with the tongue being the only articulator structure involved in their production. Distinctive features of vowels might be subtle, and the phonetic information may not be sufficient for discriminating them from other vowel sounds,12 which might result in patients being easily confused about the phonetic aspects of vowel writing. One of the possible explanations is related to the invention theory on the visuoconstructional shape of Hangul single characters. When Hangul was invented, the consonants were modeled after pictorial vocal representations; for example, ‘ㄱ /g/’ depicts the root of the tongue blocking the throat. On the other hand, the vowel graphemes are based on three geometric concepts; for example, ‘·’ is based on heaven, ‘ㅡ’ is based on earth, and ‘ㅣ’ is based on humanity. Therefore, the visuospatial image of vowel graphemes might be far more abstract than the consonant graphemes, and the abstract representation of vowel graphemes might be more difficult to recall. In addition, in terms of generating a Hangul grapheme, the 10 basic vowels were derived from the 3 basic geometric shapes. Each vowel is created by combining any or all of these three basic shapes, and any slight change to a stroke of the vowel grapheme could transform it into another vowel grapheme (e.g., ‘ㅏ/a/’→‘ㅓ/Λ/’).
In terms of the error pattern, our patients produced visuoconstructional errors as well as linguistic errors, which corroborates results from a previous study.46 The left temporal lobe involves the linguistic function and the right parietal lobe controls the visuospatial function.13 Korean stroke patients with unilateral lesions in the left hemisphere were found to exhibit linguistic errors, while those with lesions in the right hemisphere exhibited visuoconstructional errors.1415 Similarly, patients with left-hemisphere lesions showed only linguistic errors; that is, substitution and omitting errors.9101116 Based on these findings, the linguistic and visuospatial errors in the medial vowel are one of the characteristic signs of EOAD because these patients may have degeneration in both temporoparietal lobes.
The second major finding of this study was that the Hangul writing time was slower in the patient group than in the NC group (Table 3). The speed of simple line drawing did not differ significantly between the NC and EOAD groups, and so we attributed the slow writing time in EOAD patients to the slowness of factors other than motor movements, such as the cognitive processing time. Cognitive psychology studies have used the elapsed time to measure the amount of time that it takes an individual to process information. A longer processing time in patients with AD has been reported to be associated with reduced cerebral blood flow in the temporoparietal lobe.17 Indeed, many studies have found elderly AD patients to exhibit slower operational functioning compared to their healthy counterparts.181920 A study of slow processing speed in naming found that the articulation and pause times in a naming task were longer in patients with AD than in NC.17 Naming and writing tasks are not identical, but they do exhibit the following similarities: 1) patients need to integrate cognitive functions such as linguistic and motor components in order to perform these tasks, and 2) these tasks can be assessed using both moving times (e.g., articulation for naming and drawing for writing) and pause times. Thus, the slow writing time of patients with EOAD might reflect a decreased cognitive processing speed in writing.
G3 for the Hangul syllable was significantly shorter in both the NC and EOAD groups. According to the cognitive information-processing model of writing to dictation, the sound of a phoneme verbally presented by an examiner will be mapped onto an auditory engram after it has been analyzed acoustically. The phoneme is subsequently converted into a grapheme based on the correspondence between the sound and a letter. In the final stage, the grapheme is drawn by executing motor movements. When writing Hangul syllables, the initial consonant is written first and the final consonant is written last. While writing the initial consonant and the medial vowel, the patient might convert the process between the phoneme and grapheme of the target syllable. However, at the point of writing the final consonant, this converting process might have already finished. Therefore, since all phonemes of target syllables might be converted into the corresponding graphemes before writing the final consonant, only the last stage (e.g., the execution of motor movements) might be processed during the writing of the final consonant.
An alternative explanation is related to the numbers of strokes required to produce the initial consonant, medial vowel, and final consonant. When designing the writing task in this study we used only single-syllable characters with a one-to-one grapheme-to-phoneme correspondence. According to a rule of pronunciation on the seven final consonants, the mean number of strokes of the selected final consonants (e.g., ‘ㄱ’ and ‘ㄴ’) was often smaller than that of the other grapheme positions (e.g., ‘ㅊ’ and ‘ㄸ’ for initial consonants, and ‘ㅟ’ for medial vowels). The total numbers of strokes in the initial consonants, medial vowels, and final consonants were 68, 80, and 51, respectively, and so the smaller number of strokes for the final consonant may explain the shorter G3.
A particularly interesting finding was P1 being significantly longer than P2 only in the patient group (i.e., not in the NC group), which might be attributable to certain characteristics of Hangul syllabic writing. Linguistically, the vowels form the peak or nucleus of the syllables in the Korean phonological system.21 In addition, Kim et al.22 suggested that the medial vowel ‘jung-seong’ is a cardinal component of the Korean syllabic system. This is similar to alphabetic language systems; for example, the vowel is the nucleus of syllables in the English phonological system.23 However, Hangul has distinctive features from a visuospatial point of view. Thus, we primarily attributed the delayed P1 in EOAD to visuospatial factors other than the phonological aspects, such as the unique appearance of the Korean syllabic structure. For example, English uses an alphabetic system in which all of the letters within a syllable are always arranged horizontally. This means that English writers do not have to judge the visuospatial position within a syllable, especially in vowel graphemes; instead, they simply have to write the vowel next to the following graphemes. However, as mentioned above, the vowel determines and guides the writing direction in the Korean syllabic writing. While the initial consonant always appears on the upper left-hand side of the syllable and the final consonant is always placed at the bottom of the syllable, the medial vowel can be written on the right side or at the bottom of the initial consonant. Compared to the initial and final consonants, patients with EOAD might need a greater judgment ability and longer processing time for determining the visuospatial grapheme position immediately before writing the medial vowels. This would explain why P1 (the time between writing the initial consonant and the medial vowel) was significantly longer due to the visuospatial dysfunction in our patients with EOAD.
In addition, writing is not a unitary process, instead requiring the coordination of linguistic, visuospatial, and motor parameters.2425 Another possible explanation for the longer P1 is disturbance in the stages of phoneme-to-grapheme conversion and/or motor execution before writing vowels. Together these observations indicate that a longer pause time before writing the medial vowel would be another important sign of EOAD. In the future we would like to investigate whether the longer P1 is restricted to dysfunction of visuospatial judgment/planning, or whether it is also affected by other writing processes.
In conclusion, the brain changes in EOAD patients may be linked to impaired writing when they are performing the tasks of daily living and social behaviors such as signing documents, proficiency at work, and writing documents. The present results have important clinical implications, in that a vowel writing deficit could be a manifestation of brain changes as well as other signs of cognitive deterioration,26272829 and thus writing ability should be constantly monitored from the early stage of this disease. In addition, the present findings offer specific insights into the role of the vowel in Hangul syllabic writing. While the visuospatial position of vowels might not be important in English syllables, in the Hangul writing system this might be the basis of the shape of the Korean syllable and writing direction. We therefore suggest that Korean medial vowels are a cornerstone of both the linguistic and visuospatial aspects of the Korean syllabic writing system.
One limitation of this study is that we could not measure the pause time before writing the initial consonant, which we intentionally did not include in order to rule out any impairment at the auditory input level: the participants were instructed to repeat the target syllable first and to write it subsequently from dictation. Some patients repeated the target syllable several times, and thus the examiner could not control the duration before writing an initial grapheme. Advancing the present findings and overcoming this limitation will require the development of duration-measuring software. Another limitation was our inability to determine the exact location of the lesions responsible for the writing deficits that we found in our patients with EOAD. We assumed that brain areas—such as the right superior temporal gyrus, right inferior parietal lobule, right middle occipital gyrus, and right precuneus—that show more-severe hypometabolism in patients with EOAD than in patients with LOAD (based on a previous study3) are associated with the writing errors in the EOAD patients. Further studies are needed to clarify the brain-behavior relationship, since we expect that lesions at diverse loci (as typically seen in patients with EOAD) would produce lesion-specific features of writing disorders. In addition, future studies should explore the specific characteristics of Hangul writing related to both EOAD and LOAD in large numbers of patients.
Acknowledgements
This material is based upon work supported by the Ministry of Trade, Industry & Energy (MOTIE, Korea) under Industrial Technology Innovation Program (10063384).
This research was supported by Hallym University Research Fund, 2017 (HRF-201707-009).
References
1. Kim MK, Kwon OS, Kwon YB. Hangul character type clustering based on the structural shapes and the combination rules of vowels. J KIISE Softw Appl. 1998; 25:685–695.
2. Papagno C. Progressive impairment of constructional abilities: a visuospatial sketchpad deficit? Neuropsychologia. 2002; 40:1858–1867. PMID: 12207984.
3. Kim EJ, Cho SS, Jeong Y, Park KC, Kang SJ, Kang E, et al. Glucose metabolism in early onset versus late onset Alzheimer's disease: an SPM analysis of 120 patients. Brain. 2005; 128:1790–1801. PMID: 15888536.
4. Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn HJ, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011; 23:1317–1326. PMID: 21615977.
5. Kim H, Na DL. Normative data on the Korean version of the Western Aphasia Battery. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004; 26:1011–1020. PMID: 15590457.
6. Yoon JH, Kim H, Seo SW, Chin J, Kim JH, Lee KH, et al. Dysgraphia in Korean patients with Alzheimer's disease as a manifestation of bilateral hemispheric dysfunction. J Neurol Sci. 2012; 320:72–78. PMID: 22809738.
7. Seo SK. Learner's Dictionary of Korean. Seoul: Shinwon Prime;2006.
8. Glickman ME, Gray JR, Morales CJ. Combining speed and accuracy to assess error-free cognitive processes. Psychometrika. 2005; 70:405–425.
9. Cubelli R. A selective deficit for writing vowels in acquired dysgraphia. Nature. 1991; 353:258–260. PMID: 1896072.
10. Miceli G, Capasso R, Benvegnù B, Caramazza A. The categorical distinction of vowel and consonant representations: evidence from dysgraphia. Neurocase. 2004; 10:109–121. PMID: 15788250.
11. Demirci S. A selective ideational apraxic agraphia for consonants. Turk J Med Sci. 2006; 36:243–246.
12. Shin JY, Cha JE. Korean Sound System. Seoul: Hankookmoonhwasa;2003.
13. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press;1995.
14. Yoon JH, Suh MK, Kim H. Language-specific dysgraphia in Korean stroke patients. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2010; 23:247–255. PMID: 21150348.
15. Yoon JH, Shin JC, Kim DY, Suh MK, Kim H. Korean agraphia subsequent to right hemispheric lesion. Speech Sci. 2006; 13:121–132.
16. Cotelli M, Abutalebi J, Zorzi M, Cappa SF. Vowels in the buffer: a case study of acquired dysgraphia with selective vowel substitutions. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2003; 20:99–114. PMID: 20957567.
17. Warkentin S, Erikson C, Janciauskiene S. rCBF pathology in Alzheimer's disease is associated with slow processing speed. Neuropsychologia. 2008; 46:1193–1200. PMID: 18067929.
18. Pirozzolo FJ, Christensen KJ, Ogle KM, Hansch EC, Thompson WG. Simple and choice reaction time in dementia: clinical implications. Neurobiol Aging. 1981; 2:113–117. PMID: 7301037.
19. Storandt M, Beaudreau S. Do reaction time measures enhance diagnosis of early-stage dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004; 19:119–124. PMID: 14670384.
20. Gorus E, De Raedt R, Lambert M, Lemper JC, Mets T. Reaction times and performance variability in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2008; 21:204–218. PMID: 18838743.
21. Ahn SC. An Introduction to Korean Phonology. Seoul: Hanshin;1998.
22. Kim H, Na DL, Park ES. Intransigent vowel-consonant position in Korean dysgraphia: evidence of spatial-constructive representation. Behav Neurol. 2007; 18:91–97. PMID: 17538195.
23. Greenberg S. Speaking in shorthand–a syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation. Speech Commun. 1999; 29:159–176.
24. Ellis AW. Normality and Pathology in Cognitive Functions. London: Academic Press;1982.
25. Heilman KM, Valenstein E. Clinical Neuropsychology. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press;2003.
26. Altamura C, Scrascia F, Quattrocchi CC, Errante Y, Gangemi E, Curcio G, et al. Regional MRI diffusion, white-matter hyperintensities, and cognitive function in Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. J Clin Neurol. 2016; 12:201–208. PMID: 27074295.
27. Xu S, Jin X, Liu C, Jin Y, Xu Y, Chen L, et al. Investigating the prevalence of dementia and its associated risk factors in a Chinese nursing home. J Clin Neurol. 2017; 13:10–14. PMID: 27868396.
28. Lee P, Ryoo H, Park J, Jeong Y;. Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Morphological and microstructural changes of the hippocampus in early MCI: a study utilizing the Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative database. J Clin Neurol. 2017; 13:144–154. PMID: 28176504.
29. Jiménez-Huete A, Del Barrio A, Riva E, Campo P, Toledano R, Franch O. Subjective evaluation of mood and cognitive functions in a general neurology clinic: patients versus informants. J Clin Neurol. 2017; 13:259–264. PMID: 28748677.
Table 1
Orientation | Stimuli | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Vertical | 귤 | 순 | 춥 | 불 | 중 | 목 | 등 | 콩 | 쑥 | 똥 |
Mixed | 길 | 언 | 병 | 칸 | 집 | 남 | 학 | 색 | 짹 | 밴 |
왁 | 월 | 권 | 꿩 | 광 | 환 | 휠 | 쉽 | 괜 | 된 |
Table 2
Grapheme position | NC | EOAD |
---|---|---|
Initial consonant | 0.2±1.0 | 5.2±4.4 |
Medial vowel | 1.1±1.9 | 10.5±7.4 |
Final consonant | 1.0±2.2 | 6.0±8.3 |
Table 3
Parameter | NC | EOAD |
---|---|---|
G1 | 556.4±67.2 | 860.2±338.9 |
P1 | 194.8±157.7 | 618.2±410.9 |
G2 | 569.2±206.1 | 909.2±609.4 |
P2 | 191.2±98.9 | 400.4±235.3 |
G3 | 406.9±59.2 | 501.5±163.1 |
Total writing time | 1,914.7±276.1 | 3,189.5±1,422.6 |
Data are mean±standard-deviation values.
G1: writing time for the initial consonant, G2: writing time for the medial vowel, G3: writing time for the final consonant, P1: pause time between writing the initial consonant and the medial vowel, P2: pause time between writing the medial vowel and the final consonant.
EOAD: early-onset Alzheimer's disease, NC: normal controls.