Journal List > Korean J Schizophr Res > v.15(2) > 1057780

Park, Kim, Park, Kim, Kim, Koo, and Song: The Characteristics of Associative Learning of Reward Approach and Loss Aversion in Schizophrenia

Abstract

Objectives

Schizophrenia patients have deficits of prediction and learning related to dopaminergic dysfunction. It is hypothesized that there would be different characteristics in associative learning of reward approach and loss aversion between controls and patients.

Methods

Participants were 23 healthy participants and 20 out-patients fulfilling criteria for schizophrenia according DSM-IV-TR. Using a monetary incentive contingency reversal task, successful learning rates, numbers of trials and errors till learning, numbers of trials of maintaining learning, response times were measured. Characteristics of learning were compared between controls and patients.

Results

Physical anhedonia and PANSS negative symptom scores correlated with the number of trials while loss aversion was maintained. Overall correct response rates were decreased in patient group, particularly during reward approach learning. Patients required more trials and errors to learn reward approach than controls. There were no significant differences in learning performance and reaction times between groups during loss avoidance learning.

Conclusion

These results support previous reports of deficits in reward-driven learning in schizophrenia. However, anhedonia and negative symptoms were associated with the preserved function of loss avoidance learning.

Figures and Tables

Fig. 1
The mean rates and standard errors of successful learning in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. Rates of Successful Learning were divided into two dimensions and analyzed by rmANOVA. A : While reversal Learning, there were no significances (Within-group : F<0, df=1, p=0.99. Between-group F=3.95, df=1, p=0.05). B : Overall correct response rates were significant in between-group effects (F=5.58, df=1, p=0.02) but not in within-group effects (F=2.18, df=1, p=0.15). Independent T-test was using for post hoc analysis of between-group effect. Specifically, during reward phase, controls showed higher successful learning rate than control did (t=2.73, df=41, p=0.001). *: p<0.05. R to L : Reward to Loss, L to R : Loss to Reward
kjsr-15-59-g001
Fig. 2
The median and inter-quartile range of the numbers of trials and errors needed for initiating learning and trials maintained of reward and loss contingency in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. *: Between-group analysis using Mann-Whitney test revealed that the patient group needed more trials and errors to learn reward contingency (Z=-2.07, p=0.04). A : Within-group effects were analyzed by Wilcoxon-signed rank test. In the control group, significantly lesser numbers of trials and errors were needed to learn reward contingency than loss contingency (Z=-2.56, p=0.01). There were no significant within-group difference in the patient group. B : There were no between-group and within-group effects in analysis of numbers of maintained trials during reward and loss contingencies.
kjsr-15-59-g002
Fig. 3
The estimated marginal means and standard errors of the reaction times during the initiation and maintenance of reward and loss learning in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. Mixed model ANOVA was performed with a mean reaction time of correct response (453.40 ms) was used as covariates. During reward learning, within-group learning phase effect [F(1, 40)=6.39, p=0.02] and interaction between learning phase and groups were statistically significant [F(1, 40)=6.20, p=0.02].
kjsr-15-59-g003
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics
kjsr-15-59-i001

*: Statistically significant at p<0.05 (2-tailed). PANAS : Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule, PANSS : Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, TCI : Temperament and Character Inventory

References

1. Strauss GP, Frank MJ, Waltz JA, Kasanova Z, Herbener ES, Gold JM. Deficits in positive reinforcement learning and uncertainty-driven exploration are associated with distinct aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2011. 69:424–431.
crossref
2. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 2003. 26:507–513.
crossref
3. Waltz JA, Frank MJ, Robinson BM, Gold JM. Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions fromcomputational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biol Psychiatry. 2007. 62:756–764.
crossref
4. Prentice KJ, Gold JM, Buchanan RW. The Wisconsin Card Sorting impairment in schizophrenia is evident in the first four trials. Schizophr Res. 2008. 106:81–87.
crossref
5. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003. 160:13–23.
crossref
6. Frank MJ. Schizophrenia: a computational reinforcement learning perspective. Schizophr Bull. 2008. 34:1008–1011.
crossref
7. Gard DE, Kring AM, Gard MG, Horan WP, Green MF. Anhedonia in schizophrenia: distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophr Res. 2007. 93:253–260.
crossref
8. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Prentice KJ, Morris SE, Heerey EA. Reward processing in schizophrenia: a deficit in the representation of value. Schizophr Bull. 2008. 34:835–847.
crossref
9. Wächter T, Lungu OV, Liu T, Willingham DT, Ashe J. Differential effect of reward and punishment on procedural learning. J Neurosci. 2009. 29:436–443.
crossref
10. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. 2000. Washington: American Psychiatric Publishing.
11. Raven J, Court J, Raven J. Raven-Matrizen-test standard progressive matrices. 1988. Weinheim: Test Gesellschaft.
12. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Raulin ML. Scales for physical and social anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1976. 85:374.
crossref
13. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988. 54:1063.
crossref
14. Min BB, Oh HS, Lee JY. Temperament and Character Inventory(TCI) Manual. 2007. Seoul: Maumsarang.
15. Yi JS, Ahn YM, Shin HK, An SK, Joo YH, Kim SH, et al. Reliability and Validity of the Korean Version of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc. 2001. 40:1090–1105.
16. van der Gaag M, Hoffman T, Remijsen M, Hijman R, de Haan L, van Meijel B, et al. The five-factor model of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale II: a ten-fold cross-validation of a revised model. Schizophr Res. 2006. 85:280–287.
crossref
17. Blanchard JJ, Mueser KT, Bellack AS. Anhedonia, positive and negative affect, and social functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1998. 24:413–424.
crossref
18. Waltz JA, Gold JM. Probabilistic reversal learning impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophr Res. 2007. 93:296–303.
crossref
19. Lee Y, Kim YT, Seo E, Park O, Jeong SH, Kim SH, et al. Dissociation of emotional decision-making from cognitive decision-making in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007. 152:113–120.
crossref
20. Danion JM, Gokalsing E, Robert P, Massin-Krauss M, Bacon E. Defective relationship between subjective experience and behavior in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001. 158:2064–2066.
crossref
21. Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004. 5:483–494.
crossref
22. Arnt J. Pharmacological specificity of conditioned avoidance response inhibition in rats: inhibition by neuroleptics and correlation to dopamine receptor blockade. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol (Copenh). 1982. 51:321–329.
crossref
23. Borison R. The role of cognition in the risk-benefit and safety analysis of antipsychotic medication. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1996. 94:5–11.
crossref
TOOLS
Similar articles