Journal List > J Clin Neurol > v.5(2) > 1048294

Yang, Kim, Kim, Kim, Kwak, and Han: Cerebellar Hypoperfusion during Transient Global Amnesia: An MRI and Oculographic Study

Abstract

Background and purpose

Transient global amnesia (TGA) is characterized by sudden anterograde and retrograde amnesia lasting for up to 24 hours. Diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (DWI) in cases of TGA and ischemia demonstrates a high frequency of high signal intensities restricted to the hippocampus, and this has been proposed as an etiology of TGA. The aims of this study were to characterize the DWI and single-photon-emission computed tomography (SPECT) findings during the acute and recovered phases of TGA and to correlate the findings with oculomotor abnormalities.

Methods

Five consecutive patients with a clinical diagnosis of TGA underwent DWI and SPECT of the brain within 24 hours after symptom onset and again 3 days later. Eye movements were also recorded using three-dimensional video-oculography.

Results

In all patients, DWI disclosed small punctuate (1-3 mm), high-signal lesions in the lateral portion of the hippocampus. The initial SPECT also revealed hypoperfusion in the cerebellar vermis, which had recovered by the follow-up examination. Three patients showed saccadic hypermetria or impaired smooth pursuit only during the acute phase.

Conclusions

Our patients with TGA showed cerebellar vermian hypoperfusion in addition to ischemic insults to the lateral hippocampus. The oculomotor abnormalities observed in our patients support the occurrence of cerebellar dysfunction during the TGA attack.

Introduction

Transient global amnesia (TGA) is characterized by a sudden, transient memory disturbance with preserved alertness, attention, and personal identity.1,2 This memory disturbance usually resolves within 24 hours and has no long-term sequelae; recurrence rarely occurs.3 The clinical and neuropsychological spectrum of this syndrome has been characterized and several etiological factors, such as migraine, focal ischemia, venous flow abnormalities, and epileptic phenomena have been proposed.1,2 Several case reports using single-photon-emission computed tomography (SPECT) have shown transient and relative hypoperfusion of the temporal lobe during the acute phase of TGA.4-6 Several case reports using SPECT have shown hypoperfusion of the bilateral frontal cortex, thalamus, and hippocampus, and hyperperfusion of the cerebellum.7 More recently, diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (DWI) has revealed high-signal abnormalities in the hippocampus associated with TGA, thus providing strong evidence of ischemia as a causative event.4,6 Previous studies have reported that lesions in the medial temporal lobe are related to memory disturbance.5 However, no one has yet attempted to record eye movements during the acute and recovered phases of TGA and to correlate the findings with the functional brain imaging. The aims of this study were thus to characterize the findings of DWI and SPECT recorded during the acute and recovered phases of TGA and to correlate the findings with any observed oculomotor abnormalities.

Methods

Subjects

Five consecutive patients with a clinical diagnosis of TGA made at the emergency room of Seoul National University Bundang Hospital between January and June 2007 were enrolled into this study after excluding one patient in whom magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was not performed within 1 day of symptom onset. The patients comprised four women and one man aged 50-69 years (mean=60.2 years; Table 1). All five patients underwent MRI including DWI between 4 and 22 hours (mean=12 hours) after symptom onset, and all fulfilled the following six diagnostic criteria of TGA proposed by Hodges and Warlow:1
  • 1) The attack must be witnessed and information must be available from a capable observer who was present for most of the attacks.

  • 2) There must be a clear-cut anterograde amnesia during the attack.

  • 3) Clouding of consciousness and loss of personal identity must be absent and the cognitive impairment must be limited to amnesia.

  • 4) There should be no accompanying focal neurological symptom during the attack and no significant neurological sign afterward.

  • 5) Epileptic features must be absent.

  • 6) Patients with a recent head injury or active epilepsy are excluded.

Neurological examination, brain SPECT and MRI, and recording of eye movements were performed during a TGA attack in all five patients. Neurological examination, SPECT and MRI were repeated 3 days later when the patients had recovered. The patients also underwent follow-up recording of eye movements 2 weeks after symptom onset.

MRI technique and analysis

MRI was conducted using a 1.5-T unit (Intera, Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) with a sensitivity-encoding (SENSE) head coil, and head/neck synergy coil. Intervals from symptom onset to the initial MRI ranged from 4 to 22 hours (mean=12 hours). Patients initially underwent MRI using our stroke protocol, which comprised diffusion-, T1-, and T2-weighted, fluid-attenuated inversion-recovery, and conventional gradient-echo images in the transverse plane, three-dimensional time-of-flight angiography of the intracranial region, and contrast-enhanced angiography of the neck region. Subsequent diffusion- and T2-weighted imaging protocols in the coronal plane were performed perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus with thinner slices. The coronal T2-weighted imaging was performed using the fast spin-echo technique with the following parameters: matrix=256×256 interpolated to 512×512, field of view=200-220 mm, slice thickness=3 mm, TR=4,113 ms, TE=100 ms, echo-train length=24, SENSE factor=1, and number of acquisitions=3. DWI was initially performed in the transverse plane within 24 hours of TGA onset (mean=13 hours, range=4-22 hours), and then repeated 3 days later (mean=73 hours, range=71-75 hours) with four sequences: b=1,000 s/mm2 per 5-mm-thick slice, b=1,000 s/mm2 per 3-mm-thick slice, b=2,000 s/mm2 per 3-mm-thick slice, and b=3,000 s/mm2 per 3 mm-thick slice. These DWI sequences consisted of single-shot spin-echo planar imaging with the following parameters: matrix=128×128 interpolated to 256×256, field of view=220 mm, TR=5,000-12,500 ms (5,000 ms at b=1,000, 9,400 ms at b=2,000, 12,500 ms at b=3,000), TE= 60-75 ms, SENSE factor=2, and number of acquisitions=4.
Rates of lesion detection on diffusion-weighted images were compared based on the consensus of three neuroradiologists. If the lesion was positive on DWI, lesion conspicuity was graded as + or ++ (Fig. 1).6 The locations of the lesions in the hippocampus (i.e., unilateral/bilateral and head/body/tail) were evaluated primarily based on transverse DWI. The hippocampal lesions were localized more specifically on thin-section, coronal, and diffusion- and T2-weighted images. Lesion locations were arbitrarily divided into lower-medial, lateral, and upper-medial regions. According to existing knowledge of hippocampal anatomy,6,11 CA1, the largest of the four hippocampal sectors (i.e., CA1-CA4), occupies the lateral curvature of the hippocampus. Therefore, the lower-medial region corresponds approximately to the subiculum, the lateral region to CA1, and the upper-medial region to the remaining sectors, which include CA2, CA3, CA4, and the dentate gyrus.

Oculography

All five patients also underwent three-dimensional recording of eye motion with video-oculography (SMI, Teltow, Germany), which had a resolution of 0.1° and a sampling rate of 60 Hz. Initial recording of eye movements was performed with an interval of 4.3±0.6 hours (mean±SD) from TGA onset. Digitized eye position data were analyzed using MATLAB software (Mathworks, USA).
Spontaneous nystagmus with or without fixation and gaze-evoked nystagmus were recorded in the horizontal (±30°) and vertical (±20°) planes. Positional nystagmus was observed during serial changes of the positions, which included sitting, lying down, turning of the head to both sides while supine, straight head hanging, and Hallpike maneuvers in both directions. Head-shaking nystagmus was induced using a passive head-shaking maneuver.
Saccades were generated by asking the patients to follow a target moving on a light bar with ranges of ±15.0° and ±30.0°. For each saccade, latency, accuracy, and peak velocity were computed and compared with the data from 50 normal controls. The saccadic latency in the normal controls was 217.5±20.0 ms, and when the value from the patients exceeded 257.5 ms (mean±2SD), the saccades were considered delayed. The saccadic gain was computed using the following equation: saccadic gain (%)=(amplitude of the initial saccade/target amplitude)×100; the normal range for saccadic gain was 93.1±4.4%. Hypermetric saccades in the five patients were defined when the mean saccadic gain was more than 101.9% (mean+2SD) or the frequency of hypermetric saccades exceeded 13.4% (mean+2SD in the normal controls). In contrast, hypometric saccades in the patients were determined when the mean saccadic gain was less than 84.3% (mean-2SD in the normal controls) or when the frequency of gaze shift with more than two corrective saccades exceeded 7.4% (mean+2SD). To determine any abnormality in horizontal saccadic velocity, normative data were obtained from 50 normal controls by performing fixed and random saccades. The peak velocities of 2,170 saccades (0.1-29.9°) were plotted versus saccadic amplitudes, and the 95% prediction interval was calculated using the following equation: peak velocity=Vmax×(1-e-Amplitude/C), where Vmax is the asymptotic peak velocity, and C is a constant that defines the exponential rise. Data were considered abnormal when they lay outside the 95% prediction curves.
The stimulus for smooth pursuit was a light target moving in a sinusoidal pattern at peak velocities of 10°/s and 20°/s. The amplitude of target motion was 20°. For gain, the peak eye velocity was compared with the peak target velocity after eliminating saccades. Then, average gain from the accepted cycles were then compared with the values from 50 age-matched controls. In younger normal controls (age=34.9±8.0 years, range=20-49 years), the smooth pursuit gain was 0.75±0.07 and 0.73±0.07 for peak target velocities of 10°/s and 20°/s, respectively. In older normal controls (age=60.3±6.7 years, range=51-73 years), the smooth pursuit gains were 0.69±0.11 and 0.64±0.13 for peak target velocities of 10 and 20°/s, respectively. Impaired smooth pursuit was defined when the smooth pursuit gain in the patients was less than the mean-2SD of that in the age-matched normal controls at either peak target velocity.

Brain SPECT and statistical parametric mapping analysis

Brain SPECT was obtained in all patients with an average interval of 6.0±1.4 hours from the onset of TGA. SPECT images were obtained using a double-head gamma camera (ADAC Forte, Philips Medical, USA) with a low-energy, high-resolution collimator mounted on it. The subjects were injected with 30 mCi of 99mTc-ethylcysteine dimer under standard conditions of a dimly lit room with eyes closed and minimal background noise. Scanning was initiated 30 minutes after injection and data were acquired over 35 minutes. SPECT images were analyzed using statistical parametric mapping (Statistical Parametric Mapping; SPM99, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK), and implemented using Matrix Laboratory 6.5. All of the images were spatially normalized into the standard templates supplied by SPM. To minimize individual anatomical variability and increase the signal-to-noise ratio, the normalized images were smoothed by convolution with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with 16-mm Full width at half maximum prior to statistical analyses. With regard to local cerebral blood flow, the whole-brain radiological quantity was normalized using a proportional coefficient. The images of the normal controls and patients were compared using ANOVA, as were those obtained during the acute and recovered phases. The control group comprised eight age-matched, healthy volunteers (five men; age=60.0±4.7 years, range=50-65 years) who were all dextromanual and free of current or previous neuropsychiatric disease.

Results

Clinical findings

The duration of the TGA attack was 8.2±2.9 hours. The attacks were induced by physical stress in four patients: two after heavy lifting, one during fervid singing in a choir, and one after strenuous dishwashing. One patient lost her memory after crying due to emotional stress. Three patients had vascular risk factors, two had hypertension, and one had hypercholesterolemia (Table 1).

MRI

In all of the patients, DWI disclosed small punctate (1-3 mm), high-signal lesions in the hippocampus; this was the only abnormal finding on MRI. All of the lesions were unilateral (three on the left and two on the right side) and all were also clearly demonstrated on both coronal diffusion- and T2-weighted images. The lesions were located in the lateral region of the hippocampus and were matched with small areas of low signal intensity on apparent diffusion coefficient maps.6

SPECT

Individual analyses showed hypoperfusion in the cerebellar vermis and uni- or bilateral temporal lobes during the attacks in all patients. One patient (patient 5) also exhibited increased perfusion in the left frontal lobe and right corpus callosum (p<0.005, uncorrected k>100). Cerebral activity was normalized using an ANCOVA scale (Fig. 2A). The cerebellar vermian hypoperfusion had normalized 2 weeks later (Fig. 2C). Group analyses revealed hypoperfusion (p<0.005, uncorrected cluster k>100) in the cerebellar vermis during the TGA attack (Fig. 3).

Abnormal oculomotor findings

Eye movements were abnormal in all patients during the TGA attack. The most striking feature was hypermetric saccades in all patients (Fig. 2B). One patient (patient 2) demonstrated downbeat nystagmus in the primary position, and another (patient 3) displayed upbeat nystagmus during upgaze. Smooth-pursuit eye movements were mildly broken up in one patient. The vestibulo-ocular reflex gain, as judged by the head impulse test and dynamic visual acuity, was normal. The abnormal response of both eyes on the video-oculography had disappeared 2 weeks after symptom onset (Fig. 2D).

Discussion

Our patients showed cerebellar vermian hypoperfusion in addition to ischemic insults to the lateral hippocampus during the TGA attacks. Furthermore, the patients exhibited abnormal ocular movements, which are suggestive of cerebellar dysfunction, but these resolved with the normalization of cerebellar hypoperfusion. Previous studies on brain SPECT in TGA have demonstrated transient hypoperfusion of the bilateral medial temporal lobes,4 left temporal lobe,8 various cortical areas including the occipital lobe and thalamus,9 unilateral or bilateral temporal lobes,10 thalamus and angular gyrus,11 right basal ganglia, the polar area of the left temporal lobe,12 and right thalamus.13 Previously, serial brain SPECT images also revealed hypoperfusion in the bilateral medial temporal lobes during the acute phase of TGA, which normalized later. However, bilateral cerebellar hypoperfusion has not been described in TGA.
In view of the normal perfusion in the cerebral cortices, the observed cerebellar hypoperfusion cannot be explained by crossed cerebellar diaschisis. Furthermore, all patients showed abnormal oculomotor findings during the acute phase, which resolved along with the normalization of cerebellar vermian hypoperfusion. This also supports the occurrence of cerebellar dysfunction during a TGA attack.
The cerebellar vermis, sometimes called the "limbic cerebellum", plays a role in the control of affective behaviors and in the coordination of fear-related somatic and autonomic conditioned responses.14 The vermis is divided into three lobules: anterior superior (lobules I-V), posterior superior (VI and VII), and inferior posterior (VIII-X).15 The dorsal cerebellar vermis (lobules V-VII) and the fastigial nuclei are important for saccade control. Chemical inhibition of the fastigial nuclei leads to hypermetric saccades via their influence on the saccade burst generator within the brainstem. The low sampling rate of video-oculography equipment makes it unsuitable for analyzing rapid eye movements such as saccades.
The cerebellar vermis may be particularly vulnerable to stress because it has the highest density of glucocorticoid receptors during the development, exceeding even those in the hippocampus. Early stressful experience gives rise to several structural and functional neurobiological consequences, including reduced size of the corpus callosum, impaired development of the left neocortex, hippocampus, and amygdala, enhanced electrical irritability in the limbic structures, and reduced functional activity of the cerebellar vermis. Like the hippocampus, the cerebellar vermis may produce granule cells postnatally.16,17 The cerebellar vermis appears to play a role in the control of epilepsy and limbic activation. Electrical stimulation of the vermis suppresses the onset and spread of seizures.18 It has been shown that with any level of limbic symptomatology there is a marked decrease in the relative perfusion of the vermis in individuals with a history of abuse, which is indicative of a functional impairment in the activity of the cerebellar vermis.19
There is increasing awareness of the critical role played by the cerebellum in attention, language, cognition, and affect. Cognitive, linguistic, social behavioral, and emotional disruptions appear to be the primary clinical manifestation of cerebellar and vermal lesions.20 The cerebellum occupies only 10-20% of the volume of the brain, but it contains more than half of all the brain's neurons. There is new evidence that abnormalities in the cerebellar vermis are involved in a wide array of psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia, autism, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and both bipolar and unipolar depression. The cerebellar vermis exerts strong modulatory effects on the locus ceruleus, ventral tegmental area, and substantia nigra, which include neurons that form the primary norepinephrine and dopamine projections.21,22 The cerebellum not only modulates the systemic circulation, but also influences regional cerebral blood flow, and initiates long-term neuroprotection of the brain from ischemic injury, independent of its effects on the blood flow.16
These findings suggest that the vermis is important for the maintenance of psychiatric health, that it is significantly affected by early stress or neglect, and that it mediates some of the primary neurobehavioral consequences of early stress or neglect. As mentioned above, hypoperfusion of the vermis is related to the epidemiology during the TGA attack period. All patients are in a stressful condition during the attack, and it is considered that this situation affects the vermis.

Figures and Tables

Fig. 1
Diffusion-weighted images obtained from a 64-year-old female. A: Initial diffusion-weighted images obtained at 11 hours after TGA onset show no lesion at b=1,000/5 mm and b=1,000/3 mm, but a subtle bright lesion (grade +) in the right hippocampus at b=2000/3 mm and b=3,000/3 mm (arrow). B: Diffusion-weighted images obtained 3 days after symptom onset show brighter lesions (grade ++) for all sequences (arrow).
jcn-5-74-g001
Fig. 2
Comparison with eight healthy controls (p<0.005, k>100); cerebral activity was normalized using an ANCOVA scale (global=50 mL/min/100 g). A: Patient 5 showed a hypoperfusion in the cerebellar vermis, left medial temporal lobe, right corpus callosum, and bilateral frontal lobes. B: This same patient also exhibited hypermetric horizontal saccades. C: The cerebellar vermian hypoperfusion observed in patient 5 had normalized 3 days later. D: The saccadic hypermetria of both eyes on the video-oculography had disappeared 14 days after symptom onset.
jcn-5-74-g002
Fig. 3
Brain regions showing significant decreases in cerebral perfusion in five TGA patients as compared with healthy subjects. Significant voxels (p<0.005, uncorrected cluster k>100) of statistical T-maps were superimposed onto a standard MRI template that was specially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute space. The color bar presents the T-scores. The transaxial planes were cut from 38 mm below the bicommissural plane at 4-mm intervals.
jcn-5-74-g003
Table 1
Clinical characteristics of patients
jcn-5-74-i001

M: male, F: female, VOG: video-oculography, Rt: right, Lt: left.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a grant of the Korean Health 21 R&D Project, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Republic of Korea (no. A050079).

References

1. Hodges JR, Warlow CP. Syndromes of transient amnesia: towards a classification. A study of 153 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1990. 53:834–843.
crossref
2. Hodges JR, Warlow CP. The aetiology of transient global amnesia. A case-control study of 114 cases with prospective follow-up. Brain. 1990. 113:639–657.
3. Tong DC, Grossman M. What causes transient global amnesia? New insights from DWI. Neurology. 2004. 62:2154–2155.
crossref
4. Jovin TG, Vitti RA, McCluskey LF. Evolution of temporal lobe hypoperfusion in transient global amnesia: a serial single photon emission computed tomography study. J Neuroimaging. 2000. 10:238–241.
crossref
5. Sander K, Sander D. New insights into transient global amnesia: recent imaging and clinical findings. Lancet Neurol. 2005. 4:437–444.
crossref
6. Yang YS, Kim SY, Kim JH. Ischemic Evidence of Transient Global Amnesia: Location of the Lesion in the Hippocampus. J Clin Neurol. 2008. 4:59–66.
crossref
7. Pantoni L, Lamassa M, Inzitari D. Transient global amnesia: a review emphasizing pathogenic aspects. Acta Neurol Scand. 2000. 102:275–283.
crossref
8. Hodges JR. Semantic memory and frontal executive function during transient global amnesia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994. 57:605–608.
crossref
9. Schmidtke K, Reinhardt M, Krause T. Cerebral perfusion during transient global amnesia: findings with HMPAO SPECT. J Nucl Med. 1998. 39:155–159.
10. Lampl Y, Sadeh M, Lorberboym M. Transient global amnesia - not always a benign process. Acta Neurol Scand. 2004. 110:75–79.
crossref
11. Takeuchi R, Matsuda H, Yoshioka K, Yonekura Y. Cerebral blood flow SPET in transient global amnesia with automated ROI analysis by 3DSRT. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2004. 31:578–589.
crossref
12. Warren JD, Chartteron B, Thompson PD. A SPECT of the anatomy of transient global amnesia. J Clin Neurosci. 2000. 7:57–59.
13. Asada T, Matsuda H, Morooka T, Nakano S, Kimura M, Uno M. Quantitative single photon emission tomography analysis for the diagnosis of transient global amnesia: adaptation of statistical parametric mapping. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000. 54:691–694.
crossref
14. Ghelarducci B, Sebastiani L. Classical heart rate conditioning and affective behavior: the role of the cerebellar vermis. Arch Ital Biol. 1997. 135:369–384.
15. Levitt JJ, McCarley RW, Nestor PG, Petrescu C, Donnino R, Hirayasu Y, et al. Quantitative volumetric MRI study of the cerebellum and vermis in schizophrenia: clinical and cognitive correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 1999. 156:1105–1107.
16. Teicher MH, Andersen SL, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Navalta CP, Kim DM. The neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003. 27:33–44.
crossref
17. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Rajapakse JC, Vaituzis AC, Liu H, et al. Development of the human corpus callosum during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1999. 23:571–588.
crossref
18. Heath RG. Brain function in epilepsy: midbrain, medullary, and cerebellar interaction with the rostral forebrain. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1976. 39:1037–1051.
crossref
19. Anderson CM, Teicher MH, Polcari A, Renshaw PF. Abnormal T2 relaxation time in the cerebellar vermis of adults sexually abused in childhood: potential role of the vermis in stress-enhanced risk for drug abuse. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002. 27:231–244.
crossref
20. Schmahmann JD. Joseph A, editor. A new role for the cerebellum: the modulation of cognition and affect. Movement disorders. 1999. 2nd ed. Boston: Blackwell Science.
21. Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL, et al. Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology. 1998. 50:1087–1093.
crossref
22. Loeber RT, Sherwood AR, Renshaw PF, Cohen BM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Differences in cerebellar blood volume in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 1999. 37:81–89.
crossref
TOOLS
Similar articles