Journal List > Yonsei Med J > v.56(3) > 1031767

Choi, Kim, Sim, Lee, Park, Jeong, Seol, Yoon, Jhung, Park, and Cheon: Abnormal Brain Activity in Social Reward Learning in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An fMRI Study

Abstract

Purpose

We aimed to determine whether Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) would show neural abnormality of the social reward system using functional MRI (fMRI).

Materials and Methods

27 ASDs and 12 typically developing controls (TDCs) participated in this study. The social reward task was developed, and all participants performed the task during fMRI scanning.

Results

ASDs and TDCs with a social reward learning effect were selected on the basis of behavior data. We found significant differences in brain activation between the ASDs and TDCs showing a social reward learning effect. Compared with the TDCs, the ASDs showed reduced activity in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right orbitofrontal cortex, right parietal lobe, and occipital lobe; however, they showed increased activity in the right parahippocampal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that there might be neural abnormality of the social reward learning system of ASDs. Although this study has several potential limitations, it presents novel findings in the different neural mechanisms of social reward learning in children with ASD and a possible useful biomarker of high-functioning ASDs.

INTRODUCTION

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a well-known neurodevelopmental disorder that critically impairs social function. The defining symptoms of ASDs include social and communicative deficits,1 as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests.2 Among brain imaging modalities, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been proven to be a useful tool for investigating aberrant neurobiological function in social brain areas of children with ASD due to its excellent contrast properties, spatial resolution, temporal resolution, and noninvasive property; it is also helpful for observing the brain activities of subjects while the subjects perform certain task stimuli.3 Previous studies have taken multiple approaches to examining social brain abnormalities in ASDs. Many researchers have demonstrated aberrant neural mechanism in social cognition of those with ASD in facial processing studies using fMRI.4,5,6 Other prior studies have examined differences of functional connectivity between ASDs and a healthy control group. They reported that ASDs showed significantly reduced functional connectivity compared to TDCs.7,8,9
ASDs are known to be less reinforced by positive social reward such as praise or smiling toward them. Some researchers reported that deficit in social reward leaning would result in qualitative impairment of social function in children with ASD.10 There have been few functional studies related to the reward system of a social learning mechanism of ASDs. One previous study reported that the lateral prefrontal cortex was involved in the integration of cognitive and motivational information and made accurate reward-related decisions in complicated circumstances.11 Another study demonstrated that children with ASD fewer frontostriatal responses to social rewards than TDCs. These decreased responses suggested the neural impairment of social learning in children with ASD.12 There have been several attempts to find social brain abnormalities in Asian children with ASD,13,14 however none of these have included a functional brain imaging study using Korean subjects with ASD.
In this study, we aimed to demonstrate a neural mechanism related to a reward system during a social learning task with Korean children with ASD. We postulated two hypotheses. The first hypothesis was that social reward learning in children with ASD would be less complete than in the healthy controls. The second hypothesis was that the neural mechanisms of social reward systems would differ between children with ASD and TDCs and that the social reward leaning process of an aberrant neural system in children with ASD could contribute to the social deficit of ASDs. Both ASDs and TDCs were asked to perform a social learning task during a functional MRI scan in this study. During the task, subjects sought the correct answers using social cues.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

ASDs and TDCs were recruited from a child and adolescent psychiatric clinic in Severance Children's Hospital affiliated with Yonsei University College of Medicine and from an ongoing community epidemiological study in the city of Goyang in South Korea. We initially enrolled and scanned 45 children (39 boys, 6 girls); however, six boys met at least one exclusion criterion. Accordingly, we analyzed the data of 39 subjects (27 ASDs, mean age 9.9±2.5; 12 TDCs, mean age 9.2±1.8) (Table 1). For all children with ASD, the ASDs diagnosis was obtained independently by two child and adolescent psychiatrists based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) criteria.15 The following characteristics were exclusionary for ASDs: 1) a past or present history of brain damage or convulsive disorder; 2) intellectual disability or language delay; and 3) comorbid child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. All TDCs were screened by two child and adolescent psychiatrists with assessments including a psychological assessment and a neurological examination. None of the TDCs had a past or current developmental, medical, or psychiatric diagnosis. Participants were not sedated for the MRI scanning, and none were taking any psychoactive medications on the day of the scanning. This study was approved by the respective Institutional Review Boards for research with human subjects at Yonsei University Severance Hospital, where this study was performed, and at the Gachon Neuroscience Research Institute in Incheon, South Korea, where all subjects were scanned. All subjects and their parents were given a full description of the study and provided prior written informed assent and consent, respectively.

Screening and diagnostic tools

Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS)-Korean version

The SRS-K was used to screen those with ASD and TDCs before the confirmative diagnostic procedure. The severity of autism symptoms was quantified by the SRS, which consists of a 65-item rating scale commonly used with clinical and general populations. Measurements of social impairments of children were reported by their parents or teachers in their social situations. All items were rated between 0 and 3 and consisted of social awareness, characteristic autistic preoccupations/traits, social information processing, social anxiety/avoidance, and capacity for reciprocal social responses. The SRS is designed to determine a singular scale score that presents the severity of social implements in those with ASD.16 The English version of the SRS strongly correlates with DSM-IV-TR criteria scores estimated from the Autism Diagnosis Interview-Revised (ADI-R). It also shows high test-retest reliability and separates children who show pervasive developmental disorders from children who show other psychiatric disorders.17 We completed the translation and back-translation of the English version of the SRS and gathered the screening data of approximately 1280 children. In male children, the recommended cut-off score (above 70) of American children corresponded to the 97th percentile scores in the Korean sample. This indicated an 83% positive predictive value of ASDs diagnoses (in submission).

Autism Diagnosis Interview-Revised-Korean version (ADI-R-K) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Korean version (ADOS-K)

The ADI-R-K and ADOS-K were used in the diagnosis of ASDs. The ADI-R18 is an interview of caregivers and is common and semi-structured. The ADI-R provides algorithms for the 10th revision of the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD-10) and DSM-IV-TR for definitions of autism. In this interview, more than 100 items cover information about language, communication, play, social development, developmental milestones, and unusual behaviors and interests. ADI-R diagnostic criteria require a specified threshold on four algorithm domains: communication, repetitive behaviors, social interaction, and age at which certain symptoms start. The ADOS19 is also a common diagnosis tool that contains a semi-structured assessment of social interaction, communication, play, and imaginative use of materials in subjects with ASD. Compared with the ADI-R, which estimates current and earlier development of ASDs, the ADOS estimates the current status. In the ADOS, planned social activities are created in which a range of social initiations and responses are likely to appear, and communication opportunities are designed to elicit a range of interchanges. This includes play situations to allow observations of a range of imaginative activities and social role-play. It also provides standard contexts of materials, structured activities, and less structured interactions for understanding ASDs. The ADOS includes four modules which are appropriate for children and adults at different developmental and language levels. The ADI-R and ADOS have long been the gold standard for autism diagnosis.18,19 We also translated and back-translated the ADI-R and ADOS and estimated their validities in Korean children (personal communication, Dr. Y.S. Kim, December 2009).

Korean Educational Development Institute-Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised-III (KEDI-WISC)

All children with ASD and TDCs were measured for full scale, performance, and verbal IQ [full-scale IQ (FIQ), performance IQ (PIQ), and verbal IQ (VIQ)].

Scanning procedures

We performed our experiment using a 3-Tesla MRI scanner (MAGNETON Verio, Siemens, Muenchen, Germany). Functional and anatomical images were acquired through different MRI sequences. Functional images were obtained with a gradient echo single-shot echo planar image sequence [repetition time (TR)=2000 ms; echo time (TE)=30 ms; in-plane resolution=3.4×3.4 mm2; slice thickness=3 mm]. After acquiring a functional image, we also scanned a T1 weighted image to acquire an anatomical image with specific scan parameters (TR=1900 ms; TE=2.93 ms; in-plane resolution=1×1 mm2; slice thickness=1 mm). T1 weighted and functional images had the same orientation for better co-registration in three-dimensional space (3D-space).

Experimental design

An auditory discrimination task was used, where subjects had to judge the direction of the presented auditory stimulus. Linear frequency modulated (FM) tones with a duration of 600 ms served as acoustic stimuli. The FM tones differed in direction of frequency modulation (20 upward/ascending, 20 downward/descending) and in center frequency (Fc=1100-3000 Hz in steps of 100 Hz), with starting and ending frequencies calculated by Fc (Hz)±Fc (Hz)/2×Δt (s). A total of 73 FM tones were presented pseudo-randomly in an event-related design with a jittered intertrial interval of 6, 8, and 10 s. At the beginning of each trial, subjects were shown a real facial image (front view) of an unknown male person in color with a neutral facial expression. The same image was used as a control for the whole experiment. After the presentation of this image, which jittered at intervals of 6, 8, and 10 s, subjects were required to judge the direction of acoustic stimuli by pressing the correct button. They were asked to press either the right or left button; however, they needed to guess which one for ascending or descending stimulus at first trial in which they did not receive any cues for the answer. Immediately following the button press, either a positive or negative emotion shown on a real facial image of an unknown female person was presented to allow the subjects to determine whether their response was correct. The image was a frontal view with the hand in either a thumbs-up (in the case of positive emotion in the face) or thumbs-down (negative) position.

Data analysis

Behavior data analysis

The correct answer was defined as "1," and the wrong answer was defined as "0". From the first trial to the last trial, all correct answers were added to the next trial. These accumulative correct answers were calculated as percentage values. If the accumulative accuracy curve showed an increasing pattern (i.e., the gradient value of the curve was greater than zero by a regression analysis, and the final accuracy was above 50%), it was defined as a learning effect of social reward.

Functional data analysis

Functional data were analyzed using Brain Voyager QX (Brain Innovation B.V., Maastricht, the Netherlands). Anatomical data were preprocessed by a spatial normalization (Talairach transformation). Functional data were preprocessed using slice-timing correction, head-motion correction, and temporal filtering. These functional data were co-registered with each of the normalized anatomical images. We performed an individual analysis of selected subjects in the first step and a group analysis in the subsequent step. The activation map was made by the contrast of all feedback phases >base phase (non-feedback phase). In the group analysis, several brain regions were found using an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a random effect included in Brain Voyager QX. All regions of interest (ROIs) were selected based on a threshold of p<0.05. Beta values of ROIs were extracted and analyzed using a two-way ANOVA to confirm the statistical significance of all ROIs.

RESULTS

Table 1 showed no significant differences of age, FIQ, and PIQ; however, slightly significant differences of VIQ and SRS scores between the ASDs and TDCs were found (independent t-test, p<0.05). Two ASDs (ASDs with social reward learning effect and ASDs without social reward learning effect) also showed no significant differences of age, FIQ, PIQ, VIQ, SRS scores, and ADI-R scores, except for the ADI-restricted repetitive behavior (RRB) score and ADOS scores (independent t-test, p<0.05).

Behavior results of the subjects

20 children in total showed a social reward learning effect during the social reward task (Fig. 1). 15 children in the ASDs (55.6%) and 5 children in the TDCs (41.7%) showed a social reward learning effect in the behavior result (p<0.001).

Comparison of fMRI results between ASDs and TDCs showing a social reward learning effect

The fMRI results of the 20 children who showed social reward learning effects were analyzed. ASDs with a social reward learning effect showed more increased responses in the right parahippocampal gyrus (PG) [F(1,18)=7.166; p<0.05] and in the right superior temporal gyrus (STG) [F(1,18)=10.622; p<0.01] than TDCs with a social reward learning effect (Fig. 2). On the other hand, TDCs with a social reward learning effect showed significantly more responses than ASDs with a social reward learning effect in several areas, including the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [F(1,18)=5.959; p<0.05], right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [F(1,18)=8.834; p<0.01), right parietal lobe [F(1,18)=6.464; p<0.05], and occipital lobe [F(1,18)=7.616; p<0.05] (Fig. 2, Table 2).

DISCUSSION

Our results showed a significant neural difference between ASDs and TDCs with a social reward learning effect. Among the involved brain areas, the DLPFC, OFC, and parietal lobe are known as the socially-related brain areas. However, the PG and STG are related to the perception of an emotional face.4,6 The DLPFC plays a crucial role in the integration of goals and the reward system. The weak responses of the DLPFC in ASDs suggest that ASDs might show an abnormality of executive function in dealing with social tasks. This would mean that those with ASD have difficulty in regulating their activities and determining their goals. The OFC is also a well-known area involved in sensory integration and reward-related behaviors.11,20 Additionally, the parietal lobe is also involved in the integration of different stimuli; however, the fundamental roles of this area are action prediction, planning, observation, and execution.21 Therefore, the reduced responses of the OFC and parietal lobe might be associated with a dysfunction of the social reward learning process in ASDs. This would mean that those with ASD use a different strategy of reward system during the social learning task compared to TDCs.
Interestingly, the ASDs showed more activation of the STG and PG. The STG is the brain area primarily responsible for perceiving emotional faces,4 processing eye-gaze,22 and detecting novel sounds.23 The increased response of the STG suggests that those with ASD are more sensitive than TDCs during the performance of a task. This is consistent with the findings of a previous study, which showed more activation in the left STG in the ASDs than in the TDCs during a sentence comprehension task.24 The PG is involved in the involuntary reactivation of the contextual fear memory that results in avoidance25 and is also a part of the limbic system, which was mentioned as a socially-related area of the brain.26 The hyper-activation of the PG is related to abnormal responses to social feedback. This dysfunction was reported in a previous study, which showed stronger connectivity of the STG and PG in ASDs than in TDCs.9 We found that the ASDs and the TDCs used different neural systems during the social reward learning task, and these neural mechanisms might reflect fundamental neurobiological changes in the brains of the subjects.
This study still has several potential limitations. The first is that we used a small sample size. Although every single subject fully practiced the task before the scanning procedure, they showed some difficulty in paying attention constantly during the actual scanning procedure. Some children complained that they had not understood the rules of the task due to confusion when pressing buttons after the beep sound. These problems made the subjects move and perform poorly during the task. The second limitation is related to the type of subjects observed. We used only high-functioning ASDs for this study, as lower-functioning ASDs with intellectual disabilities could not perform and understand social reward learning tasks. Therefore, our results cannot be generalized to all ASDs. We will need to specifically observe the various types of ASDs in future fMRI research with a much simpler task.
Despite these limitations, there still are novel findings in our investigation related to the neural mechanism of high-functioning ASDs. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first fMRI study investigating a neural mechanism in the social reward learning system of Asian ASDs. These results could be used as a biomarker of high-functioning ASDs. In addition, this study is also valuable due to its focus on young children with high-functioning ASDs. Although this report does not include a cross-cultural comparison, it provides a first step toward understanding the neural mechanism of the social reward system in children with ASD.
In conclusion, we found that there might be a neural abnormality of the social reward learning system in ASDs. Despite several limitations, our study presented novel findings in terms of the differences of the neural mechanism between ASDs and TDCs with a social learning effect and a potential useful biomarker of high-functioning children with ASD.

Figures and Tables

Fig. 1

Behavioral results of the subjects. (A) Comparison between the ASDs and TDCs who showed a social reward learning effect. There was no significant difference between the two groups. (B) Comparison between different ASDs subgroups. LE, subgroup with a social reward learning effect; nLE, subgroup without a social reward learning effect; ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TDC, typically developing control.

ymj-56-705-g001
Fig. 2

Different brain activation maps of ASDs and TDCs with a social reward learning effect. (A) Different brain activation maps of the ASDs> the TDCs contrast. The parahippocampal gyrus and superior temporal cortex were observed (ANOVA analysis with random effect GLM, p<0.01, uncorrected). (B) Different brain activation maps of the TDCs> the ASDs contrast. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex and parietal lobe were observed (ANOVA analysis with random effect GLM, p<0.01, uncorrected). ANOVA, analysis of variance; GLM, general linear model; ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TDC, typically developing control; PG, parahippocampal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex.

ymj-56-705-g002
Table 1

Demographic and Psychosocial Data of the Subjects

ymj-56-705-i001
ASD (n=27) TDC (n=12) LE ASD (n=15) nLE ASD (n=12)
AGE (yrs) (SD) 9.9 (2.5) 9.1 (1.6) 9.5 (2.2) 10.4 (2.9)
FIQ (SD) 105.9 (14.7) 103.7 (10.2) 106.7 (15.4) 105.1 (14.5)
PIQ (SD) 99.1 (17.0) 97.4 (13.6) 100.2 (15.6) 97.9 (19.5)
VIQ (SD) 111.8 (15.6) 108.4 (7.3) 113.7 (15.2) 109.6 (16.5)
SRS (SD)* 86.4 (21.4) 22.3 (12.0) 81.5 (20.4) 94.1 (22.2)
ADI-R social skill (SD) 15.2 (5.7) 14.8 (5.2)
ADI-R communication (SD) 14.6 (5.2) 14.0 (3.5)
ADI-R repetitive behavior (SD) 4.7 (2.4) 4.1 (1.1)
ADI-R birth threshold (SD) 2.7 (1.4) 2.4 (1.6)
ADOS communication (SD)* 3.2 (1.3) 3.9 (1.9)
ADOS social skill (SD) 7.8 (1.3) 7.3 (2.1)
ADOS creativity (SD) 0.6 (0.7) 1.0 (0.7)
ADOS repetitive behavior (SD) 1.8 (1.4) 0.7 (0.7)

SD, standard deviation; ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TDC, typically developing control; FIQ, full-scale IQ; PIQ, performance IQ; VIQ, verbal IQ; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale; ADI-R, Autism Diagnosis Interview-Revised; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; LE, a group with social reward Learning effect; nLE, a group without social reward learning effect.

*Independent t-test, p<0.05.

Table 2

Comparison of Activated Brain Areas during the Task in the ASDs and TDCs with a Social Reward Learning Effect

ymj-56-705-i002
Comparison Hemisphere Region BA Talaiach coordinates z-value p value
x y z
ASD>TDC Right Parahippocampal gyrus BA19 23 -50 0 7.166 0.015
Right Superior temporal gyrus BA41 46 -36 9 10.622 0.004
TDC>ASD Right Occipital lobe BA18 -6 -89 -13 7.616 0.013
Right Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex BA9 26 32 25 5.959 0.025
Right Orbitofrontal cortex BA11 33 41 -6 8.834 0.008
Right Parietal lobe BA7 24 -57 43 6.464 0.020

ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TDC, typically developing control; BA, brodmann area.

By two-way analysis of variance with a random effect.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by a research grant from the Korean Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health & Welfare, Republic of Korea [Grant number: HI12C0021 (A120029), HI12C0245 (A120296)] and by a faculty research grant from Yonsei University College of Medicine for 2010 (6-2010-0139).

Notes

The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

References

1. Seol KI, Song SH, Kim KL, Oh ST, Kim YT, Im WY, et al. A comparison of receptive-expressive language profiles between toddlers with autism spectrum disorder and developmental language delay. Yonsei Med J. 2014; 55:1721–1728.
crossref
2. Samson AC, Phillips JM, Parker KJ, Shah S, Gross JJ, Hardan AY. Emotion dysregulation and the core features of autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014; 44:1766–1772.
crossref
3. Dichter GS. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of autism spectrum disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012; 14:319–351.
crossref
4. Dalton KM, Holsen L, Abbeduto L, Davidson RJ. Brain function and gaze fixation during facial-emotion processing in fragile X and autism. Autism Res. 2008; 1:231–239.
crossref
5. Doyle-Thomas KA, Goldberg J, Szatmari P, Hall GB. Neurofunctional underpinnings of audiovisual emotion processing in teens with autism spectrum disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2013; 4:48.
crossref
6. Greimel E, Nehrkorn B, Fink GR, Kukolja J, Kohls G, Müller K, et al. Neural mechanisms of encoding social and non-social context information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2012; 50:3440–3449.
crossref
7. von dem Hagen EA, Stoyanova RS, Baron-Cohen S, Calder AJ. Reduced functional connectivity within and between 'social' resting state networks in autism spectrum conditions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013; 8:694–701.
crossref
8. Jones TB, Bandettini PA, Kenworthy L, Case LK, Milleville SC, Martin A, et al. Sources of group differences in functional connectivity: an investigation applied to autism spectrum disorder. Neuroimage. 2010; 49:401–414.
crossref
9. Monk CS, Peltier SJ, Wiggins JL, Weng SJ, Carrasco M, Risi S, et al. Abnormalities of intrinsic functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage. 2009; 47:764–772.
crossref
10. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16:231–239.
crossref
11. Sakagami M, Watanabe M. Integration of cognitive and motivational information in the primate lateral prefrontal cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007; 1104:89–107.
crossref
12. Scott-Van Zeeland AA, Dapretto M, Ghahremani DG, Poldrack RA, Bookheimer SY. Reward processing in autism. Autism Res. 2010; 3:53–67.
crossref
13. Gunji A, Goto T, Kita Y, Sakuma R, Kokubo N, Koike T, et al. Facial identity recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders revealed by P300 analysis: a preliminary study. Brain Dev. 2013; 35:293–298.
crossref
14. Fan YT, Chen C, Chen SC, Decety J, Cheng Y. Empathic arousal and social understanding in individuals with autism: evidence from fMRI and ERP measurements. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014; 9:1203–1213.
crossref
15. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association;2000.
16. Constantino JN, Davis SA, Todd RD, Schindler MK, Gross MM, Brophy SL, et al. Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003; 33:427–433.
17. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Genetic structure of reciprocal social behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 2000; 157:2043–2045.
crossref
18. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994; 24:659–685.
crossref
19. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000; 30:205–223.
20. Eshel N, Nelson EE, Blair RJ, Pine DS, Ernst M. Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia. 2007; 45:1270–1279.
crossref
21. Teixeira S, Machado S, Velasques B, Sanfim A, Minc D, Peressutti C, et al. Integrative parietal cortex processes: neurological and psychiatric aspects. J Neurol Sci. 2014; 338:12–22.
crossref
22. Murphy ER, Foss-Feig J, Kenworthy L, Gaillard WD, Vaidya CJ. Atypical Functional Connectivity of the Amygdala in Childhood Autism Spectrum Disorders during Spontaneous Attention to Eye-Gaze. Autism Res Treat. 2012; 2012:652408.
crossref
23. Ross B. A novel type of auditory responses: temporal dynamics of 40-Hz steady-state responses induced by changes in sound localization. J Neurophysiol. 2008; 100:1265–1277.
crossref
24. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004; 127(Pt 8):1811–1821.
crossref
25. Paquette V, Lévesque J, Mensour B, Leroux JM, Beaudoin G, Bourgouin P, et al. "Change the mind and you change the brain": effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on the neural correlates of spider phobia. Neuroimage. 2003; 18:401–409.
crossref
26. Pagani M, Manouilenko I, Stone-Elander S, Odh R, Salmaso D, Hatherly R, et al. Brief Report: alterations in cerebral blood flow as assessed by PET/CT in adults with autism spectrum disorder with normal IQ. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012; 42:313–318.
crossref
TOOLS
Similar articles