Journal List > Korean J Physiol Pharmacol > v.19(6) > 1026059

Kim, Kim, Lee, Kong, and Kaang: Mind Bomb-2 Regulates Hippocampus-dependent Memory Formation and Synaptic Plasticity

Abstract

Notch signaling is a key regulator of neuronal fate during embryonic development, but its function in the adult brain is still largely unknown. Mind bomb-2 (Mib2) is an essential positive regulator of the Notch pathway, which acts in the Notch signal-sending cells. Therefore, genetic deletion of Mib2 in the mouse brain might help understand Notch signaling-mediated cell-cell interactions between neurons and their physiological function. Here we show that deletion of Mib2 in the mouse brain results in impaired hippocampal spatial memory and contextual fear memory. Accordingly, we found impaired hippocampal synaptic plasticity in Mib2 knock-out (KO) mice; however, basal synaptic transmission did not change at the Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapses. Using western blot analysis, we found that the level of cleaved Notch1 was lower in Mib2 KO mice than in wild type (WT) littermates after mild foot shock. Taken together, these data suggest that Mib2 plays a critical role in synaptic plasticity and spatial memory through the Notch signaling pathway.

ABBREVIATIONS

Mib

Mind bomb

RING domain

Really Interesting New Gene domain

WT

wild type

KO

knock-out

LTP

long-term potentiation

E-LTP

early-phase LTP

L-LTP

late-phase LTP

LTD

long-term depression

NMDAR

N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor

LFS

low frequency stimulation

TBS

theta burst stimulation

HFS

high frequency stimulation

GluN2B

GluRepsilon2

pGluN2B

phosphorylated GluRepsilon2

INTRODUCTION

The Notch signaling pathway is an intracellular signal transduction mechanism, which is highly conserved throughout multicellular eukaryotic species [1]. To date, five Notch ligands and four Notch receptors have been identified in mammalian cells. As these are all type 1 transmembrane proteins, Notch signaling can occur only between physically adjacent cells [2]. During the development of invertebrate and vertebrate species, Notch signaling plays a critical role in regulating cell fate and integrating other developmental cues [3456]. During the development of the mammalian central nervous system (CNS), Notch signaling maintains the progenitor cell population and regulates its maturation [7]. Besides the well-known developmental role of Notch signaling, recent studies report that Notch signaling in the CNS is also involved in synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory [7891011]. These studies show that LTP and LTD were both impaired in hippocampal slices from transgenic mice with reduced Notch level, suggesting that Notch signaling is involved in modification of CA 1 synapses during stimulation [11]. Not only Notch 1 itself but also its ligand Jagged 1 were found at the synapse and activated by neuronal activity. When Notch 1 was conditionally deleted in postnatal hippocampus, LTP and LTD were impaired, leading to learning and short-term memory deficits [12].
The mib gene was originally found in zebrafish; it encodes the protein Mind bomb (Mib), which mediates Notch signaling by ubiquitinating the Notch ligand Delta and promoting its endocytosis [13]. Kong and his colleagues identified the mouse homologue of mib, and created knock-out (KO) transgenic mouse lines for Mind bomb-1 (Mib1) and its paralogue Mind bomb-2 (Mib2) [1415]. The E3 ubiquitin ligase activity of both Mib1 and Mib2 is mediated by the C-terminal Really Interesting New Gene (RING) domain, and Mib1 and Mib2 interact with Xenopus Delta (XD) via their N-terminal region [13]. When expression levels are compared in mice, Mib2 is highly expressed in adult tissues and is less abundant in embryos; however, Mib1 is highly expressed in both adult and embryotic tissues [16]. Even though zebrafish Mib and Mib2, which are orthologous to mouse Mib1 and Mib2, have redundant roles in zebrafish development [17], the different expression patterns of Mib1 and Mib2 suggest that mouse Mib1 and Mib2 may have different roles.
Even though the role of Notch signaling in development and synaptic plasticity is already known [12], the role of Mib2 in learning and memory is still unclear. In our previous study, we reported that conditional knockout of Mib1 in mature neurons of the mouse forebrain result in impaired synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory [18]. To assess whether Mib2 has a different function compared with Mib1, we used Mib2 KO mice and examined their learning and memory functions. We found impaired hippocampus-dependent long-term memory, such as contextual fear memory, in the Mib2 KO mice compared with their WT littermates. Similarly, acute hippocampal slices prepared from Mib2 KO mice exhibited impairments in various forms of LTP. When we measured protein levels in the hippocampus of these mice, we found that the level of cleaved Notch1 was lower in Mib2 KO mice than in their WT littermates after mild foot shock. These results suggest that Mib2-mediated Notch signaling is essential for regulating synaptic plasticity and memory formation in the hippocampus.

METHODS

Mice

We used 8~15-week-old male Mib2 KO and wild type (WT) littermates on the C57BL/6N genetic background for behavioral experiments. Mice were kept on a 12-h light: dark cycle, and behavioral experiments were performed during the light phase. Food and water were provided ad libitum. The Animal Care and Use Committee of Seoul National University approved the animal protocols used in this study.

Behavioral tests

For all behavioral tasks, 8~15 weeks old male mice were used. Before performing the task, mice were placed on a shelf for at least 40 minutes for accommodation.

Morris water maze task

Mice were handled for 3 minutes 7 consecutive days before the task. After the handling, mice were put into a gray opaque cylinder-shaped tank (140 cm diameter, 100 cm height) in a room with multiple spatial cues including a water tap and a computer desk, where the experimenter sat. The tank was divided into 4 virtual quadrants, and a Perspex platform (10 cm diameter) was placed at the center of a quadrant (TQ). The tank was filled with water (20~22℃), until the water level was 1 cm higher than the platform, and white paint was added to make the water opaque. Before the 1st trial on training day 1, mice were placed on the platform for 30 seconds. On the training days, mice were released at the edge of the maze facing towards the inner wall of the tank, and were trained to reach the platform. If mice were not able to reach the platform in 60 seconds, they were taken out of the maze. The releasing point was chosen randomly in every trial. When the mice failed to reach the platform, they were guided to or placed on the platform, and were rescued from the maze after 10 seconds. When the mouse successfully reached the platform, and stayed on it for more than 1 second, it was rescued from the maze after 10 seconds. Each mouse performed 4 trials per training day. The interval between trials 1 and 2 or trials 3 and 4 was 1 minute, and the interval between trials 2 and 3 was 30~45 minutes. Each mouse performed 4 training trials per day for 5 consecutive days.

Contextual fear conditioning

Prior to contextual fear conditioning, we handled each mouse for 3 minutes on 4 consecutive days. For contextual fear conditioning, the mice were placed in a chamber for 3 minutes. After 148 seconds, they were presented with an unexpected foot shock for 2 seconds (0.4 mA). Mice were returned to the chamber for testing their fear memories after 24 hours. Freezing (immobile posture except for respiration) level was measured automatically by a computer program (FreezeFrame; Coulbourn).

Elevated plus maze task

During the test, mice were placed at the center of the plus maze, and their movement was tracked for 5 minutes by a tracking program (EthoVision 3.1; Nodulus) under fluorescent light. The maze was made of white Plexiglas, and its arms were 150-cm long. The two opposing closed arms had additional walls with a height of 20 cm.

Open field task

Mice were put in a square opaque box (40×40×40 cm). The tasks were performed under dim light, and mice were tracked with a tracking program (EthoVision 3.1; Nodulus) for 10 minutes.

Electrophysiology

Field excitatory postsynaptic potential recordings were performed as described previously [19]. Using a manual tissue chopper, transverse hippocampal slices (400-µm thick) were prepared from 4~5-week-old mice for measuring the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) mediated long-term depression (LTD) or from 8~12-week-old mice (male and female) for other protocols. Animals were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane (Hana Medical), decapitated, and their brains were removed and sectioned. Hippocampal slices were maintained in an interface chamber at 28℃ oxygenated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 and perfused (1~1.5 ml/min) with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF; 124 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 10 mM glucose, 2 mM CaCl2, 2 mM MgSO4). Slices were incubated in the interface chamber for at least 2 hours. After this recovery period, extracellular field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were recorded from the Schaffer collaterals (SC) of the CA1 region using a glass pipette electrode filled with ACSF (1 MΩ). The SC were stimulated every 30 seconds using concentric bipolar electrodes (MCE-100; Kopf Instruments) placed at the CA1 region. Field potentials were amplified, low-pass filtered (GeneClamp 500; Axon Instruments), and then digitized (NI PCI-6221; National Instruments) for measurement. Data were monitored, analyzed online, and reanalyzed offline using the WinLTP program (WinLTP Ltd., winltp.com, The University of Bristol, UK). For measuring long-term potentiation (LTP) and LTD, stimulation intensity was adjusted to produce a fEPSP slope that is approximately 40% of the slice's maximum slope. Two successive responses elicited twice per minute at this test stimulation intensity were averaged, and then expressed relative to the normalized baseline. After a stable baseline was recorded, we induced LTP by high-frequency stimulation (HFS; 100 Hz stimulation, 1 second, test intensity), LTD by low-frequency stimulation (LFS; 900 stimuli, 1 Hz, test intensity), and LTP by theta burst stimulation (TBS; 3×TBS, 1 second each, test intensity). Data were monitored, analyzed online, and reanalyzed offline using the WinLTP program.

Western blot

For western blot analysis, hippocampi of 8~12 weeks old mice were used. Mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated; then, their hippocampi were removed and frozen immediately in liquid nitrogen and stored at -80℃ until lysis with a lysis buffer (1.6% SDS, 10 mM TrisCl; pH 6.8). Protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche 11873580001) and protein phosphatase inhibitor cocktail (Roche 04906845001) were also added to the lysis buffer. Hippocampal tissue was lysed in 400 µl lysis buffer with the QIAGEN TissueLyser LT (50 Hz, 3 min). For analysis of cleaved Notch1, mice were given a mild foot shock (0.5 mA, 2 sec) using the same protocol as for contextual fear conditioning. Fifteen micrograms of the protein samples were run by electrophoresis, and the blotted membranes were immunolabeled with antibodies against phospho-GluRepsilon2 (pGluN2B, Sigma-Aldrich, M2442), GluRepsilon2 (GluN2B, Neuromab, 75-097), Actin (Sigma-Aldrich, A2066), Tubulin (NeuroMab 75-330), cleaved Notch1 (CST, 2421) and Mib2 (Abnova H00142678-M01A). Mib1 antibody was gift from Dr. P. Gallagher.

RESULTS

Hippocampus-dependent memory was abnormal in Mib2 KO mice

Before examining whether Mib2 deletion affects learning and memory, we tested if Mib2 KO mice exhibit abnormal anxiety. In the open field and elevated plus maze tasks, locomotive activity and anxiety level were comparable between Mib2 KO and WT mice (Fig. 1A~C). Then we tested whether mice have an intact hippocampal function using the Morris water maze [20]. In this task, mice have to learn and remember the location of a hidden platform beneath the water surface using spatial cues in the room. In the training trials, the latency to reach the hidden platform was significantly longer in the KOs than in their WT littermates (Fig. 1D). These results suggest that deletion of Mib2 impairs spatial learning in mice.
Next, we examined the mice using contextual fear conditioning test, which is another hippocampus-dependent memory task. In the training session, mice were placed in a conditioning chamber, where mild foot shocks were delivered. Then, the trained mice were exposed to the same conditioning chamber to assess their freezing behavior 24 hours after the training. Consistent with the result of the Morris water maze, Mib2 KO mice exhibited significantly less freezing behavior than WT mice (Fig. 1E), demonstrating that hippocampal learning and memory are impaired in Mib2 KO mice.

Basal synaptic transmission and LTD was normal in Mib2 KO mice

To identify the mechanism responsible for the memory deficits caused by Mib2 deletion, we examined the electrophysiological properties of Mib2 KO mice with extracellular field recordings at the SC-CA1 synapses in acute hippocampal slices. The input-output relationship and paired-pulse ratio were indistinguishable in WT and Mib2 KO mice (Fig. 2A and 2B), demonstrating that the basal synaptic transmission is intact in Mib2 KO mice. Since spatial memory of Mib2 KO mice was impaired in the NMDAR-dependent [21] water maze task, we examined the NMDAR-dependent LTD at the SC-CA1 pathway by delivering LFS (900 pulses at 1 Hz) using acute hippocampal slices. We found no significant difference in the level of LTD between genotypes (Fig. 2C), indicating that the genetic deletion of Mib2 does not affect NMDAR-LTD.

Mib2 KO mice had stimulation protocol dependent deficits in E-LTP and L-LTP

LTP at the SC-CA1 synapses plays an important role in spatial learning and memory. Mutant mice with impaired LTP often exhibit deficits in the hippocampus-dependent learning and memory [9]. Consistent with the results of behavioral tasks, which investigated the hippocampus-dependent memory, Mib2 KO mice had an abnormal TBS-induced early-phase LTP (E-LTP) (Fig. 3A). In contrast, the potentiation level of HFS (100 Hz) induced E-LTP was normal in Mib2 KO mice compared to WT controls in the last 5 minutes (Fig. 3B).
Interestingly, this protocol-dependent deficit of synaptic plasticity in Mib2 KO mice reversed during the late-phase LTP (L-LTP), which is a de novo protein synthesis dependent form of synaptic plasticity [222324]. When we induced L-LTP in the hippocampal slices by delivering four pulses of high frequency tetanus with 5 minutes intervals, L-LTP was significantly decreased in Mib2 KO mice compared to their WT littermates (Fig. 3C). However, when we induced L-LTP by delivering TBS three times with 10 minutes intervals, the potentiation level during the last 5 minutes was similar in theMib2 KO and WT littermates (Fig. 3D). These results suggest that Mib2 selectively regulates E-LTP and protein synthesis-dependent L-LTP at hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses.
These protocol-dependent deficits of synaptic plasticity in Mib2 KO mice may be explained by the difference between the two stimulation protocols. TBS and HFS both induce LTP, but they resemble two separate prominent rhythms in the brain: theta rhythm of 5 to 7 Hz and high frequency gamma rhythm of 50 to 100 Hz. While TBS is known to more accurately replicate the stimulus pattern of the naturally occurring rhythmic activity in hippocampus in vivo [25], increasing evidences show that they do not share intracellular mechanisms when inducing LTP. TBS-induced LTP requires ERK MAPK activity [26] while HFS-induced LTP does not [27]. A more recent study compared the two protocols and showed that although they share mechanisms like actin polymerization, TBS-induced LTP involves calpain-1 activation and suprachiasmatic nucleus circadian oscillatory protein degradation, while HFS-induced LTP does not. Instead, HFS requires adenosine A2 receptors and PKA [28]. TBS and HFS may represent different physiological conditions of learning and some cases have been reported where transgenic mice show LTP deficits in only one protocol [29]. Therefore, the protocol-dependent LTP and L-LTP deficit in Mib2 KO mice suggests that Mib2 is important in selective synaptic plasticity pathways.

Mib2 KO mice had a normal GluN2B level, but a diminished Notch signaling

What is the molecular mechanism that caused memory impairments and synaptic plasticity deficits in Mib2 KO mice? To answer this question, we prepared hippocampal lysates from Mib2 KO and WT littermates. In the hippocampus of Mib2 KO mice, compared to the hippocampi of their WT littermates, Mib2, but not Mib1 was specifically deleted (Fig. 4A). A previous study reported that Mib2 ubiquitinates the GluN2B subunit of the NMDAR receptor [30]. However, we found no difference in the levels of pGluN2B and GluN2B between Mib2 KO and WT littermates (Fig. 4B~D). Next, we measured the level of cleaved Notch1 in the hippocampi of Mib2 KO and WT mice without foot shock and 1 hour after mild foot shock (Fig. 4E). Mib2 KO mice had a reduced level of hippocampal cleaved Notch1 after mild foot shock compared with their WT littermates (Fig. 4F). These results show that the deficits in Mib2 KO mice might be a result of impaired activity-dependent Notch signaling.

DISCUSSION

In the early developmental stages of Drosophila and zebrafish, Mib2 mediates Notch signaling by ubiquitinating the Notch ligand Delta [16]. Despite its abundant expression, the function of Mib2 in the adult mouse brain was previously unknown. In this study, we report that Mib2 plays an important role in hippocampus-dependent memory formation and synaptic plasticity by regulating Notch signaling.
Previous studies suggested that Notch signaling is involved in learning and memory formation [81031], synaptic plasticity such as LTP and LTD [11], and neuronal activity-dependent immediate early gene expression [12]. Notch1 null heterozygous knockout and conditional knockout mice exhibit normal locomotion, but impaired spatial memory [1231]. Similarly, our study showed impaired spatial learning of Mib2 KO mice in the Morris water maze test.
Are these defects in the hippocampus-dependent behavioral tasks and synaptic plasticity of Mib2 KO mice caused by impaired Notch signaling in the hippocampus? The basal level of cleaved Notch1 was similar in WT and Mib2 KO littermates, suggesting that Mib2 does not mediate basal Notch signaling. However, Mib2 KO mice had a lower level of cleaved Notch1 compared to WT mice 1 hour after a mild foot shock. Considering that hippocampal neuronal activity and synaptic plasticity are critical for contextual fear memory formation [32], Mib2 might mediate Notch signaling in the adult hippocampus during hippocampus-dependent learning and neuronal activity.
The input-output curve and paired pulse ratio confirmed that the basal transmission is not impaired in Mib2 KO mice. However, Mib2 KO mice clearly showed deficits in two different forms of LTP in a stimulation protocol dependent manner. As Notch is a transcription co-activator, and Mib2 ubiquitinates a Notch ligand [16], the deficit in L-LTP in Mib2 KO mice suggests that Mib2 might regulate L-LTP via transcription. Additionally, the deficient E-LTP in Mib2 KO mice suggests that potentiation of glutamate receptor responses might be disrupted in synapses occurring in these mice [33]. Therefore, Mib2 might be important in the formation and maintenance of synaptic plasticity.
Mib2 has been reported to ubiquitinate the GluN2B subunit of the NMDAR receptor in the postsynaptic site [30]. Therefore, we investigated if deficits observed in Mib2 mice during behavioral experiments and electrophysiological recordings were due to the dysregulation of GluN2B caused by the absence of Mib2. However, we found similar levels of pGluN2B and GluN2B in Mib2 KO and WT mouse hippocampi. In our study, we used whole hippocampal lysates. If Mib2 ubiquitinates GluN2B subunits only at the postsynaptic, but not at the extrasynaptic site, the misregulation of postsynaptic GluN2B ubiquitination could be obscured by the non-affected extrasynaptic GluN2B population. In another report [16], Mib2 was shown to regulate the Notch signaling pathway by ubiquitinating the ligand protein Delta. Although Notch signaling is mainly known to regulate development [34], recent studies showed that it is also activated by neuronal activity and regulates synaptic plasticity [12]. Because Notch signaling in neuronal cells regulates activity-dependent synaptic plasticity [1135], the impaired memory and synaptic plasticity in Mib2 KO mice might be due to a decreased Notch signaling. More importantly, our previous report showed that conditional knock-out (cKO) of Mib1, a Mib2 paralogue, in mature neurons of the mouse forebrain results in impaired Notch signaling [18]. To test if Mib2 is also involved in Notch signaling in the adult mouse brain, we measured the level of cleaved Notch1 in the hippocampi of Mib2 KO and WT mice. Unlike the forebrain of Mib1 cKO mice [18], Mib2 KO and WT mouse hippocampi had a comparable level of cleaved Notch1. However, when cleaved Notch1 levels were measured 1 hour after mild foot shock, Mib2 KO mouse hippocampi contained a lower level of cleaved Notch1 than WT hippocampi. Mib2 is highly expressed in adult brain tissues, but shows a lower expression level in embryonic brain tissues; however, Mib1 is constantly expressed at a high level [16]. Therefore, our data indicate that Mib1 is involved in basal Notch signaling in the adult brain, while Mib2 is involved in neural activity-dependent Notch signaling. However, since Mib2 KO mice are conventional KO mice line, the possibility that normal levels of basal Notch signaling was maintained by a compensatory mechanism in Mib2 mice still remains.
The lower level of cleaved Notch1 after fear conditioning and the impaired synaptic plasticity in Mib2 KO mice implicates that Mib2 might mediate Notch signaling during activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus. Our study suggests that the absence of Mib2 leads to decreased hippocampal Notch signaling during learning, and thus causes impaired hippocampus-dependent learning, memory formation, and synaptic plasticity in Mib2 KO mice.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by the grant of National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Korean Government (MSIP, National Honor Scientist Program). T.K. is supported by the POSCO TJ Park Foundation.

Notes

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION: H.L. and B.K.K. designed the experiments. H.L. and S.M. conducted the electrophysiological experiments; H.L. and T.K. performed the behavioral experiments; T.K. performed the western blot experiments; T.K., S.M., Y.Y.K, and B.K.K. analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript.

ABBREVIATIONS

Mib

Mind bomb

RING domain

Really Interesting New Gene domain

WT

wild type

KO

knock-out

LTP

long-term potentiation

E-LTP

early-phase LTP

L-LTP

late-phase LTP

LTD

long-term depression

NMDAR

N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor

LFS

low frequency stimulation

TBS

theta burst stimulation

HFS

high frequency stimulation

GluN2B

GluRepsilon2

pGluN2B

phosphorylated GluRepsilon2

References

1. Gazave E, Lapébie P, Richards GS, Brunet F, Ereskovsky AV, Degnan BM, Borchiellini C, Vervoort M, Renard E. Origin and evolution of the Notch signalling pathway: an overview from eukaryotic genomes. BMC Evol Biol. 2009; 9:249. PMID: 19825158.
crossref
2. Sorkin A, Von Zastrow M. Signal transduction and endocytosis: close encounters of many kinds. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol. 2002; 3:600–614. PMID: 12154371.
crossref
3. Greenwald I. LIN-12/Notch signaling: lessons from worms and flies. Genes Dev. 1998; 12:1751–1762. PMID: 9637676.
4. Artavanis-Tsakonas S, Rand MD, Lake RJ. Notch signaling: cell fate control and signal integration in development. Science. 1999; 284:770–776. PMID: 10221902.
crossref
5. Mumm JS, Kopan R. Notch signaling: from the outside in. Dev Biol. 2000; 228:151–165. PMID: 11112321.
crossref
6. Baron M. An overview of the Notch signalling pathway. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2003; 14:113–119. PMID: 12651094.
crossref
7. Yoon K, Gaiano N. Notch signaling in the mammalian central nervous system: insights from mouse mutants. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8:709–715. PMID: 15917835.
crossref
8. Ge X, Hannan F, Xie Z, Feng C, Tully T, Zhou H, Xie Z, Zhong Y. Notch signaling in Drosophila long-term memory formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004; 101:10172–10176. PMID: 15220476.
crossref
9. Grant SG, O'Dell TJ, Karl KA, Stein PL, Soriano P, Kandel ER. Impaired long-term potentiation, spatial learning, and hippocampal development in fyn mutant mice. Science. 1992; 258:1903–1910. PMID: 1361685.
10. Sargin D, Botly LC, Higgs G, Marsolais A, Frankland PW, Egan SE, Josselyn SA. Reprint of: disrupting Jagged1-Notch signaling impairs spatial memory formation in adult mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013; 105:20–30. PMID: 23850596.
crossref
11. Wang Y, Chan SL, Miele L, Yao PJ, Mackes J, Ingram DK, Mattson MP, Furukawa K. Involvement of Notch signaling in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004; 101:9458–9462. PMID: 15190179.
crossref
12. Alberi L, Liu S, Wang Y, Badie R, Smith-Hicks C, Wu J, Pierfelice TJ, Abazyan B, Mattson MP, Kuhl D, Pletnikov M, Worley PF, Gaiano N. Activity-induced Notch signaling in neurons requires Arc/Arg3.1 and is essential for synaptic plasticity in hippocampal networks. Neuron. 2011; 69:437–444. PMID: 21315255.
crossref
13. Itoh M, Kim CH, Palardy G, Oda T, Jiang YJ, Maust D, Yeo SY, Lorick K, Wright GJ, Ariza-McNaughton L, Weissman AM, Lewis J, Chandrasekharappa SC, Chitnis AB. Mind bomb is a ubiquitin ligase that is essential for efficient activation of Notch signaling by Delta. Dev Cell. 2003; 4:67–82. PMID: 12530964.
crossref
14. Koo BK, Lim HS, Song R, Yoon MJ, Yoon KJ, Moon JS, Kim YW, Kwon MC, Yoo KW, Kong MP, Lee J, Chitnis AB, Kim CH, Kong YY. Mind bomb 1 is essential for generating functional Notch ligands to activate Notch. Development. 2005; 132:3459–3470. PMID: 16000382.
crossref
15. Koo BK, Yoon MJ, Yoon KJ, Im SK, Kim YY, Kim CH, Suh PG, Jan YN, Kong YY. An obligatory role of mind bomb-1 in notch signaling of mammalian development. PLoS One. 2007; 2:e1221. PMID: 18043734.
16. Koo BK, Yoon KJ, Yoo KW, Lim HS, Song R, So JH, Kim CH, Kong YY. Mind bomb-2 is an E3 ligase for Notch ligand. J Biol Chem. 2005; 280:22335–22342. PMID: 15824097.
crossref
17. Zhang C, Li Q, Lim CH, Qiu X, Jiang YJ. The characterization of zebrafish antimorphic mib alleles reveals that Mib and Mind bomb-2 (Mib2) function redundantly. Dev Biol. 2007; 305:14–27. PMID: 17331493.
crossref
18. Yoon KJ, Lee HR, Jo YS, An K, Jung SY, Jeong MW, Kwon SK, Kim NS, Jeong HW, Ahn SH, Kim KT, Lee K, Kim E, Kim JH, Choi JS, Kaang BK, Kong YY. Mind bomb-1 is an essential modulator of long-term memory and synaptic plasticity via the Notch signaling pathway. Mol Brain. 2012; 5:40. PMID: 23111145.
crossref
19. Choi JH, Park P, Baek GC, Sim SE, Kang SJ, Lee Y, Ahn SH, Lim CS, Lee YS, Collingridge GL, Kaang BK. Effects of PI3Kγ overexpression in the hippocampus on synaptic plasticity and spatial learning. Mol Brain. 2014; 7:78. PMID: 25373491.
crossref
20. Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat Protoc. 2006; 1:848–858. PMID: 17406317.
crossref
21. Hayashi Y, Nabeshima Y, Kobayashi K, Miyakawa T, Tanda K, Takao K, Suzuki H, Esumi E, Noguchi S, Matsuda Y, Sasaoka T, Noda T, Miyazaki J, Mishina M, Funabiki K, Nabeshima Y. Enhanced stability of hippocampal place representation caused by reduced magnesium block of NMDA receptors in the dentate gyrus. Mol Brain. 2014; 7:44. PMID: 24893573.
crossref
22. Karpova A, Mikhaylova M, Thomas U, Knöpfel T, Behnisch T. Involvement of protein synthesis and degradation in long-term potentiation of Schaffer collateral CA1 synapses. J Neurosci. 2006; 26:4949–4955. PMID: 16672670.
crossref
23. Sanhueza M, Lisman J. The CaMKII/NMDAR complex as a molecular memory. Mol Brain. 2013; 6:10. PMID: 23410178.
crossref
24. Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. Expression of NMDA receptor-dependent LTP in the hippocampus: bridging the divide. Mol Brain. 2013; 6:5. PMID: 23339575.
crossref
25. Lynch MA. Long-term potentiation and memory. Physiol Rev. 2004; 84:87–136. PMID: 14715912.
crossref
26. Siarey RJ, Kline-Burgess A, Cho M, Balbo A, Best TK, Harashima C, Klann E, Galdzicki Z. Altered signaling pathways underlying abnormal hippocampal synaptic plasticity in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. J Neurochem. 2006; 98:1266–1277. PMID: 16895585.
crossref
27. Selcher JC, Weeber EJ, Christian J, Nekrasova T, Landreth GE, Sweatt JD. A role for ERK MAP kinase in physiologic temporal integration in hippocampal area CA1. Learn Mem. 2003; 10:26–39. PMID: 12551961.
crossref
28. Zhu G, Liu Y, Wang Y, Bi X, Baudry M. Different patterns of electrical activity lead to long-term potentiation by activating different intracellular pathways. J Neurosci. 2015; 35:621–633. PMID: 25589756.
crossref
29. Costa AC, Grybko MJ. Deficits in hippocampal CA1 LTP induced by TBS but not HFS in the Ts65Dn mouse: a model of Down syndrome. Neurosci Lett. 2005; 382:317–322. PMID: 15925111.
crossref
30. Jurd R, Thornton C, Wang J, Luong K, Phamluong K, Kharazia V, Gibb SL, Ron D. Mind bomb-2 is an E3 ligase that ubiquitinates the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor NR2B subunit in a phosphorylation-dependent manner. J Biol Chem. 2008; 283:301–310. PMID: 17962190.
crossref
31. Costa RM, Honjo T, Silva AJ. Learning and memory deficits in Notch mutant mice. Curr Biol. 2003; 13:1348–1354. PMID: 12906797.
crossref
32. Ressler KJ, Paschall G, Zhou XL, Davis M. Regulation of synaptic plasticity genes during consolidation of fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2002; 22:7892–7902. PMID: 12223542.
crossref
33. Kelleher RJ, Govindarajan A, Tonegawa S. Translational regulatory mechanisms in persistent forms of synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2004; 44:59–73. PMID: 15450160.
crossref
34. Williams R, Lendahl U, Lardelli M. Complementary and combinatorial patterns of Notch gene family expression during early mouse development. Mech Dev. 1995; 53:357–368. PMID: 8645602.
crossref
35. de Bivort BL, Guo HF, Zhong Y. Notch signaling is required for activity-dependent synaptic plasticity at the Drosophila neuromuscular junction. J Neurogenet. 2009; 23:395–404. PMID: 19863270.
crossref
Fig. 1

Mib2 KO mice show impaired hippocampus-dependent learning and memory. (A) WT and Mib2 KO mice showed normal levels of basal anxiety in the open field test (WT, n=8; KO, n=11; two-way ANOVA, interaction between genotype and zone, F2,17=1.082, p=0.350). (B) The total distance moved during the open field test was similar in WT and Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=8; KO, n=11; unpaired t-test, p=0.562). (C) Mib2 KO mice showed a normal level of anxiety in the elevated plus maze (WT, n=7; KO, n=8; two-way ANOVA, interaction between genotype and arms, F1,13=0.002, p=0.966). (D) Learning curve during 5 training days of the Morris water maze task shows the latency required for the mice to reach the platform (WT, n=9; KO, n=9; two-way ANOVA, genotype, F1,16=7.191, *p<0.05). (E) Freezing levels of WT and Mib2 KO mice before (pre-training) and after (retrieval) contextual fear conditioning (WT, n=8; KO, n=11; two-way ANOVA, interaction between genotype and conditioning, F1,17=14.39, **p<0.01). All graphs represent mean±SEM. Abbreviations: WT, wild type; KO, Knockout.

kjpp-19-515-g001
Fig. 2

General electrophysiological characteristics of Mib2 KO mice. (A) Input-output relationship at the SC-CA1 synapses was not different between Mib2 KO and WT littermates (WT, n=8; KO, n=8; two-way ANOVA, p=0.9993). (B) Paired pulse ratio was normal in Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=9; KO, n=8; two-way ANOVA, p=0.8546). (C) NMDAR-dependent LTD was similar in WT and Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=6; KO, n=9; unpaired t-test, p=0.8788). Abbreviations: SC, Schaffer collaterals; WT, wild type; KO, knock-out; LTD, long-term depression; NMDAR, N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor.

kjpp-19-515-g002
Fig. 3

E-LTP and L-LTP are selectively impaired in Mib2 KO mouse depending on the induction protocol. (A) TBS-induced E-LTP was impaired in Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=12; KO, n=9; unpaired t-test, *p<0.05). (B) HFS-induced E-LTP at the SC-CA1 synapses was not different in WT and Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=7; KO, n=8; unpaired t-test, p=0.9001). (C) The potentiation level of HFS-induced L-LTP was different during the last 5 minutes in WT and Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=3; KO, n=4; unpaired t-test, *p<0.05). (D) The potentiation level of TBS-induced L-LTP was different during the last 5 minutes in WT and Mib2 KO mice (WT, n=11; KO, n=9; unpaired t-test, p=0.2498). Abbreviations: SC, Schaffer collaterals; WT, wild type; KO, knock-out; E-LTP, early-phase long-term potentiation; L-LTP, late-phase long-term potentiation; TBS, theta burst stimulation; HFS, high frequency stimulation

kjpp-19-515-g003
Fig. 4

Mib2 KO mice have a normal GluN2B level, but a reduced Notch signaling. (A) Protein expression of Mib2, but not Mib1, was downregulated in Mib2 KO mouse hippocampi. (B~D) pGluN2B and GluN2B levels were comparable in Mib2 KO and WT littermates. (C) pGluN2B levels normalized to the total GluN2B levels (WT, n=5; KO, n=4; unpaired t-test, p=0.490). (D) GluN2B levels normalized with tubulin (WT, n=5; KO, n=4; unpaired t-test, p=0.494). (E, F) Cleaved Notch1 level in the hippocampus was comparable in WT and Mib2 KO littermates in the home cage condition. Mib2 KO mice had a lower level of cleaved Notch1 than their WT littermates after a mild foot shock (WT, n=5; KO, n=4; unpaired t-test, *p<0.05). Abbreviations: WT, wild type; KO, knock-out; GluN2B, GluRepsilon2; SM, size marker.

kjpp-19-515-g004
TOOLS
Similar articles